Article Text

PDF

Anosognosia for hemiplegia after a brainstem haematoma: a pathological case
  1. SERGE BAKCHINE,
  2. ISABELLE CRASSARD
  1. Centre du Langage et de Neuropsychologie, Hôpital La Salpêtrière, Paris, France
  2. Laboratoire de Neuropathologie R Escourolle, Hôpital La Salpêtrière, Paris, France
  1. Dr Serge Bakchine, Centre du Langage et de Neuropsychologie, Fédération de Neurologie, Hôpital de La Salpêtrière, 47 bvd de l’hôpital, F 75651 Paris Cedex 13, France.
  1. DANIELLE SEILHAN
  1. Centre du Langage et de Neuropsychologie, Hôpital La Salpêtrière, Paris, France
  2. Laboratoire de Neuropathologie R Escourolle, Hôpital La Salpêtrière, Paris, France
  1. Dr Serge Bakchine, Centre du Langage et de Neuropsychologie, Fédération de Neurologie, Hôpital de La Salpêtrière, 47 bvd de l’hôpital, F 75651 Paris Cedex 13, France.

Statistics from Altmetric.com

The pathogenesis of anosognosia for hemiplegia (AHP) remains unclear, but recent theories have involved global cognitive deterioration or more modular mechanisms.1-3 Persistent AHP is usually found in left hemiplegic patients with large right frontoparietal cortical lesions or with subcortical damage, specially to the perithalamic regions.1 2 4 Anosognosia for motor deficit is exceptionally reported in “non-hemispheric” damage and is related to a simultaneous confusional state.5 6 We report a patient with severe and prolonged anosognosia for left hemiplegia occurring after a right pedonculopontine haemorrhage. A possible explanation for this AHP might be the association of a left complete somatosensory deficit secondary to brainstem lesion with a global cognitive impairment due to histologically established Alzheimer’s disease. This case, to our knowledge, is unique in the literature and it raises interesting questions on the origin of AHP.

An 83 year old right handed woman was admitted to hospital because she was found confused with a left hemiplegia. She had an history of hypertension treated with a salidiuretic. She had lived alone since the death of her husband, 15 years ago, and was taking good care of herself, despite a moderate impairment of memory for recent events noticed by her daughter since the past two years. On admission, she was lethargic with a blood pressure of 185/105 mm Hg, a complete proportional left motor and sensory deficit, a discrete right peripheral facial paresis, a conjugate gaze deviation to the left, and swallowing difficulties. Brain CT showed a right lateralised pontine haemorrhage. The mental confusion decreased progressively and disappeared at the end of the second week after onset, allowing a full participation in examination. During the third and fourth weeks after onset, the left hemiplegia remained stable. The patient was unable to detect any type of sensory stimuli when applied on her left side, whereas answers elicited from the right side were normal. There was no sign of left spatial hemineglect or left hemiasomatognosia: the patient performed a perfect midline bisection, and she was able to point on command with her right hand to various parts of her extrapersonal space and of her body. She expressed no delusion of absence or ownership of body parts. She scored from 16 to 19/30 on Folstein’s mini mental state examination (MMSE), depending on her fatigue. At the same time she was totally unaware of her deficit, as we showed repeatedly with Starkstein’s anosognosia questionnaire.2 She never reported her sensorimotor deficit spontaneously. When asked about the reasons for her stay in hospital, she would evoke a check up ordered by her physician. Non-oriented questions about her health would obtain similar answers. She could see no reason that could restrain her ability to walk, except some degree of fatigue. To any specific question directed towards the presence of a sensory or motor deficit, she would always report none. When asked to move her left arm or leg, she would move the right limb instead. When the deficit was directly shown to her, she would never acknowledge it or show any emotional reaction. She would usually say that she was too exhausted to move any more. At the same time, when asked if a person with one paralysed side would be able to walk, she would answer that, of course, it was impossible with such a deficit. From weeks 5 to 7 after onset, the patient‘s condition continued to improve. The MMSE score was consistantly about 22/30. She scored 117/144 on the Mattis dementia rating scale (attention32/37; initiation/perseveration 26/49; construction 3/6; conceptualisation 33/39; memory 20/25). Somatosensory deficit was unchanged, whereas the left motor deficit improved slightly in her left arm. Brain MRI performed during the fifth week after onset, showed no associated focal hemispheric lesion other than the haematoma, but bilateral white matter hyperintensities (T2 sequences) in the high frontal periventricular regions. During the same period, the patient never reported her deficit in the following situations: casual conversation, following a general question, or after specific questions regarding her limb strength or feeling. When confronted with the reality of the deficit, she would sometimes admit that she was not able to walk, and that her left limbs were “ a little bit stiff”. A few minutes later she would manifest no concern about her deficit. Conversely, she repeatedly questioned the resident about her blood pressure, and complained about the inconvenience due to the loss of her glasses. We did not assess with a formal questionnaire8the presence of anosognosia to cognitive deficits, but we noticed that the patient acknowledged (but never reported spontaneously) mild memory disturbances and cognitive slowing. She twice received a psychiatric assessment that showed a slightly blunted affect, with no depression or anxiety. The patient died suddenly during the eighth week after onset. Necropsy showed that death was due to bilateral pulmonary embolism. Her brain weighed 1210 g. The left temporal lobe showed a moderately atrophic aspect. There was no focal cortical or subcortical lesion in the cerebral hemispheres. A 1cm wide haematoma was found in the right rostral pons extending from the caudal midbrain to the middle pons (figure). We performed the usual histological staining (haematoxylin-eosin, Bodian silver, Luxol fast blue, and Congo red) and an immunohistochemical analysis of Aβ and tau proteins. Lesions of Alzheimer’s disease were found in all the cortical areas, characterised by numerous Aβ positive amyloid plaques. Neurofibrillary tangles were rare in the isocortices, but were numerous within hippocampic (mostly in CA2) and entorhinal cortices on both sides. Neuronal loss was more severe in the left CA1 subarea, but was symmetric elsewhere.

Serial cuts of the brainstem showing a haematoma extending from the right ventrolateral part of the caudal midbrain to the right upper midpart of the pons.

We describe a patient with a mild Alzheimer’s disease who had a lateralised brainstem lesion and developed subsequently a severe and long lasting AHP, in all points similar to that described in right hemispheric damages. This AHP contrasted with a very mild anosognosia to cognitive deficits and normal concerns with other medical problems. We found no associated cortical or subcortical infarction, specially in the right parietal, frontal, or perithalamic regions, nor myelinic pallor such as described in microvascular hypertensive pathology. The brainstem lesion obviously caused the patient’s motor and somatosensory deficits, but cannot by itself explain the AHP. Hemisensory loss (specially proprioception) is necessary for persistent AHP, but not sufficient.1 The initial confusional state did not last long enough to cause the AHP, as proposed in other reports.5 6 A feed forward deficit3 is not an appropriate explanation, as the lesion site would not cause a motor neglect (postulated as the main factor of AHP in this hypothesis), and because the AHP remained almost unchanged when the motor deficit started to improve. The patient’s death prevents us from knowing whether a greater recovery would have overcome the anosognosia. The patient showed no behavioural particularity that could contribute to the AHP;7 she presented no visuoverbal confabulation nor delusion, and psychiatric examination disclosed no depression or specific attitude toward illness.

We think that this unusual AHP could result from the association in the same patient of a stroke and Alzheimer’s disease through a special pattern of cognitive deficit. Levine et al have reported several patients with prestroke dementia who sustained hemiplegia and had no AHP.1 These patients, however, had no hemisensory loss. Starkstein et al 2 have shown that cognitive deficits are not necessary to produce AHP but could constitute a contributing factor in some patients. In our patient, is not clear whether the AHP was due to a global or to a specific cognitive impairment. We think that a global reduction of attentional resources cannot explain the AHP in this case, given the specificity of the anosognosia. We could not relate AHP to a degraded body scheme, a hemispatial neglect, or a hemiasomatognosia. The memory deficit was mild, without confabulatory tendency, and it would not explain the AHP by a difficulty of remembering the newly acquired hemiplegia. Some theories of unawareness posit that AHP could be, at least partly, related to some mental inflexibility or to deficits in self monitoring and internal representation.1 2 Such deficits are usually associated with damage to the frontal lobes or to frontal-subcortical circuits. In our patient, the analysis of the Mattis scale subscores showed a prominent frontal dysexecutive syndrome. This peculiar cognitive pattern may have been of some importance in the appearance of AHP in the patient. Starkstein et al 9 have proposed two domains of anosognosia in Alzheimer’s disease, cognitive and behavioural. Based on this very unusual case, we think that anosognosia to non-cognitive non-behavioural deficits might represent a third domain that should be assessed systematically in patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease with a stroke. Such studies might be very useful to the understanding of mechanisms of anosognosia.

Acknowledgments

We thank Professor Ch Duyckaerts for his most helpful comments.

References

View Abstract

Request permissions

If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.