Skip to main content
Log in

Mechanisms underlying the generation of averaged modified trajectories

  • Published:
Biological Cybernetics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this work we have studied what mechanisms might possibly underlie arm trajectory modification when reaching toward visual targets. The double-step target displacement paradigm was used with inter-stimulus intervals (ISIs) in the range of 10–300 ms. For short ISIs, a high percentage of the movements were found to be initially directed in between the first and second target locations (averaged trajectories). The initial direction of motion was found to depend on the target configuration, and on D: the time difference between the presentation of the second stimulus and movement onset. To account for the kinematic features of the averaged trajectories two modification schemes were compared: the superposition scheme and the abort-replan scheme. According to the superposition scheme, the modified trajectories result from the vectorial addition of two elemental motions: one for moving between the initial hand position and an intermediate location, and a second one for moving between that intermediate location and the final target. According to the abort-replan scheme, the initial plan for moving toward the intermediate location is aborted and smoothly replaced by a new plan for moving from the hand position at the time the trajectory is modified to the final target location. In both tested schemes we hypothesized that due to the quick displacement of the stimulus, the internally specified intermediate goal might be influenced by both stimuli and may be different from the location of the first stimulus. It was found that the statistically most successful model in accounting for the measured data is based on the superposition scheme. It is suggested that superposition of simple independent elemental motions might be a general principle for the generation of modified motions, which allows for efficient, parallel planning. For increasing values of D the inferred locations of the intermediate targets were found to gradually shift from the first toward the second target locations along a path that curved toward the initial hand position. These inferred locations show a strong resemblance to the intermediate locations of saccades generated in a similar double-step paradigm. These similarities in the specification of target locations used in the generation of eye and hand movements may serve to simplify visuomotor integration.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Andersen RA, Essick GK, Siegel RM (1985) Encoding of spatial location by posterior parietal neurons. Science 230: 456–458.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Atkeson CG, Hollerbach JM (1985) Kinematic features of unrestrained vertical arm movements. J. Neurosci. 5: 2318–2330.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Aslin RN, Shea SL (1987) The amplitude and angle of saccades to double-step target displacements. Vision Res 27: 1925–1942.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Barrett NC, Glencross DJ (1988) The double step analysis of rapid manual aiming movements. Q J Exp Psychol 40A: 299–322.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Becker W, Jurgens R (1979) An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli. Vision Res 19: 967–983.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Flash T, Henis E (1991) Arm trajectory modification during reaching toward visual targets. J Cogn Neurosci 3: 220–230.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Flash T, Hogan N (1985) The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model. J Neurosci 7: 1688–1703.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Flash T, Henis E, Inzelberg R, Korczyn AD (1992) Timing and sequencing of human arm trajectories: normal and abnormal motor behavior. J Hum Mov Sci 11: 83–100.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Flashner H, Beuter A, Arabyan A (1988) Fitting mathematical functions to joint kinematics during stepping: implications for motor control. Biol Cybern 58: 91–99.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Georgopoulos AP, Schwartz AB, Kettner RE (1986) Neuronal population coding of movement direction. Science 233: 1416–1419.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Georgopoulos AP, Lurito JT, Petrides M, Schwartz AB (1989) Mental rotation of the neuronal population vector. Science 243: 234–236.

    Google Scholar 

  12. Gielen CCAM, Van den Heuvel PJM, Van Gisbergen JAM (1984) Coordination of fast eye and arm movements in a tracking task. Exp Brain Res 56: 154–161.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Hening W, Favila M, Ghez C (1988) Trajectory control in targeted force impulses. V. Gradual specification of response amplitude. Exp Brain Res 71: 116–128.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Henis E (1991) Strategies underlying arm trajectory modification during reaching toward visual targets. PhD Dissertation, Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot, Israel.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Henis E, Flash T (1989) Mechanisms subserving arm trajectory modification. Perception 18: 495.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Henis E, Flash T (1991) A mechanism for arm trajectory modification induced by stimuli presented in a quick succession. The 14th Annual Meeting of the European Neuroscience Association, September 7–14, Cambridge, England.

  17. Henis E, Flash T (1992) A computational mechanism to account for averaged modified hand trajectories. In: Moody JE, Hanson SJ, Lipmann RP (eds) Advances in neural information processing systems 4. Part IX. Morgan Kaufmann, San Mateo, pp 619–626.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Henis E, Stanford T, Sparks D (1992) A computational model for modified saccade trajectories. Soc Neurosci Abstr 18: 700.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Hoff B, Arbib MA (1992) A model of the effects of speed, accuracy and perturbation on visually guided reaching. In: Caminiti R (eds) Control of arm movement in space: neurophysiological and computational approaches. Exp Brain Res Suppl 22, Springer-Verlag, Berlin, pp 285–306.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Hollerbach JM, Flash T (1982) Dynamic interactions between limb segments during planar arm movement. Biol Cybern 44: 67–77.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Keele SW (1981) Behavioral analysis of movement. In: Brooks VB (eds) Handbook of Physiology, Sect 1. The nervous system, Vol 2 Motor Control. American Physiological Society, Bethesda, pp 1391–1414.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Marquardt DW (1963) An algorithm for least-squares estimation of non-linear parameters. J SIAM 11: 431–441.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Morasso P (1981) Spatial control of arm movements. Exp Brain Res 42: 223–227.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Morasso P, Mussa-Ivaldi FA (1982) Trajectory formation and handwriting: a computational model. Biol Cybern 45: 131–142.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Munhall K, Lofqvist A (1987) Gestural aggregation in speech. PAW Rev 2: 13–15.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Munoz DP, Pelisson D, Guitton D (1991) Movement of neural activity on the superior colliculus motor map during gaze shifts. Science 251: 1358–1360.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Poulton EC (1981) Human manual control. In: Brooks VB (eds) Handbook of Physiology, Sect 1. The nervous system, Vol 2. Motor control. American Physiological Society, Bethesda, pp 1337–1389.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Rosenbaum DA, Engelbrecht SE, Bushe MM, Loukopoulos LD (1993) Knowledge model for selecting and producing reaching movements. JMotor Behav 25: 217–227.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Soechting JF, Lacquaniti F (1981) Invariant characteristics of a pointing movement in man. J Neurosci 1: 710–720.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Soechting JF, Lacquaniti F (1983) Modification of trajectory of a pointing movement in response to a change in target location. J Neurophysiol 49: 548–564.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Sternberg S, Monsell S, Knoll RL, Wright CE (1978) The latency and duration of rapid movement sequences. In: Stelmach GE (eds) Information processing in motor control and learning. Academic Press, New York, pp 117–152.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Uno Y, Kawato M, Suzuki R (1989) Formation and control of optimal trajectory in human multijoint arm movement. Biol Cybern 61: 89–101.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Van Gisbergen JAM, Van Opstal AJ (1989) Models. In: Wurtz RH, Goldberg ME (eds) The neurobiology of saccadic eye movements. Elsevier, Amsterdam.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Van Gisbergen JAM, Van Opstal AJ, Roebroek JGH (1987) Stimulusinduced midflight modification of saccade trajectories. In: O'Regan JK, Levy-Schoen A (eds) Eye movements: from physiology to cognition. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 27–36.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Van Opstal AJ, Van Gisbergen JAM (1990) Role of monkey superior colliculus in saccade averaging. Brain Res 79: 143–149.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Van Sonderen JF (1989) The generation of motor programmes for goal directed movements. PhD Thesis, Utrecht.

  37. Van Sonderen JF, Gielen CCAM, Denier Van der Gon JJ (1989) Motor programmes for goal directed movements are continuously adjusted according to changes in target location. Exp Brain Res 78: 139–146.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

The majority of this work was performed when E.A.H. was with the Department of Applied Mathematics and Computer Science of the Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot, Israel.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Henis, E.A., Flash, T. Mechanisms underlying the generation of averaged modified trajectories. Biol. Cybern. 72, 407–419 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00201416

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00201416

Keywords

Navigation