Progressive impairment of constructional abilities: a visuospatial sketchpad deficit?

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00072-6Get rights and content

Abstract

The case of a patient, PC, with an impairment of constructional abilities due to a progressive degenerative disease is described. Ideomotor apraxia was also present. PC showed difficulties in all tasks requiring a “manipulation” of a visual model or requiring a visual model to address a specific movement or a choice among alternatives. The spatial component of mental imagery was also severely impaired. The conclusion was that the patient presented a deficit of the rehearsal component of the visuospatial sketchpad (VSSP). This produced an impairment of spatial learning, but spatial retrograde memory was preserved, as well as object recognition. The most prominent sites of atrophy were located in the parietal lobe bilaterally. A 3-year follow-up is reported, showing a progressive impairment of verbal abilities.

Introduction

In recent years, there have been a large number of case reports of patients showing a progressive breakdown in language abilities, with sparing of other cognitive functions [40]. There are also descriptions of progressive dementia in which the prominent features are visuo-perceptual and spatial impairments [12], [18], [31], as well as two reports of patients with progressive degeneration of the right temporal lobe and progressive prosopoagnosia [15], [36]; finally, two cases of progressive visual agnosia have been described [9].

Visuo-perceptual impairment is reported in patients with Lewy bodies dementia [26]: these patients are particularly impaired in size and form discrimination, overlapping figure identification and visual counting.

Disorders of constructional abilities are very common in the course of degenerative dementia [17] and they may also be the first symptom [12], or the most severe [21]. However, no attempt has been made in these cases to understand the “functional” locus of the impairment. In contrast, cases with selective cognitive deficits may provide an interesting contribution to the development of cognitive models of visuospatial processing. In one of such cases, it has been shown that mental representation of horizontal and vertical spatial relations in an egocentric co-ordinate system are functionally dissociated [21].

There are two main problems regarding constructional apraxia (CA). The first concerns the fact that CA can, indeed, be caused by a variety of cognitive deficits: visuo-perceptual, mental representation, motor implementation or executive disorders. The second problem is related to methodological issues, because CA has been assessed in different ways: copying designs; drawing from memory; arranging sticks in a pattern; building three-dimensional models; block design. Even if all of these tasks require some basic common abilities, they nevertheless differ in other important aspects, such as the load of general intelligence or the involvement of grapho-motor abilities. Recently, the study of CA has become the study of drawing, more precisely of copying. Copying requires different stages to be carried out. Several models have been proposed [20], but essentially, they all include three main stages. During the preliminary stage, a search for an interpretative hypothesis of the model is performed: on the one hand, the individual tries to identify in the stimulus objects that have already been drawn in the past, and at the same time he/she analyses the spatial relationships between elements of the picture and those between the picture and the paper on which it is drawn. During this stage a visual representation of the model is stored in a visual short-term memory system. This stage requires visuo-perceptual abilities and short-term memory. In the second stage, the elements identified by this analysis are processed in order to formulate a drawing plan. It has also been proposed that a subject can retrieve separate parts of the model, which are known configurations (such as squares, circles, etc.), stored in a sort of “constructional lexicon” [1]. This stage requires mental imagery and retrieval from long-term memory. Then, in the third, executive stage, the plan is preserved in a short-term memory system for as long as is necessary to complete its translation onto paper, due to activation of motor programmes [20].

Visuospatial short-term memory in constructional tasks has not received much attention, but from the description above, it is evident that a short-term memory system plays a role. I will refer to the visuospatial sketchpad (VSSP) component of working memory (WM) [2]. The VSSP is assumed to maintain and manipulate visual information and to be involved in visual imagery. Logie [23] suggests that the VSSP can be divided in two sub-components (in analogy with the phonological loop): the visual cache and the inner scribe, working in partnership. The visual cache is thought to deal with information that is visual in nature, such as form and colour, and to be closely linked to the activities of the visual perceptual system. The inner scribe, in contrast, is proposed to handle information about movement sequences and, in a manner somewhat analogous to the sub-vocal rehearsal process of the phonological loop, to refresh the contents of the visual cache. It is obvious, therefore, that in case of a damage of the visual cache or the inner scribe, a constructional task would not be performed. Baddeley and Andrade [4] suggest that the maintenance of information by active rehearsal is necessary for imagery. They assume that the regeneration process is part of the function of the slave systems.

A number of studies, both psychological and neuropsychological, now exist to suggest separable subsystems of visuospatial memory for dealing with spatial information (such as the location of an object) and visual information (such as appearance) [13], [16], [32]. Recent research has hypothesised that WM is not a form of transit lounge that acts to hold perceptual input on its way to long-term memory, but as a workspace [24].

Here, the case of a woman who showed a severe CA, due to a progressive degenerative disease, is described. A mild degree of ideomotor apraxia was also present, while oral apraxia was absent. The functional locus of impairment is discussed in the frame of the WM model [2].

With regard to neural correlates, all data from the literature seem to confirm that CA is generally due to lesions encroaching upon the posterior (parieto-temporo-occipital) regions [17]. In the past, the qualitative differences observed between the constructional performance of right and left brain-damaged subjects led to advance the hypothesis that an executive, planning defect subsumed constructional disturbances in left brain-damaged patients, while visuospatial deficits could be responsible for the disorder in right brain-damaged patients [17]. This distinction, however, has proved to be inconsistent [19].

Recently, four patients have been described with a progressive degenerative pathology, showing early visuospatial problems, agraphia of a predominantly peripheral type and difficulty with bimanual tasks [34]: neuroimaging disclosed bilateral parietal lobe atrophy and hypoperfusion, which was out of proportion to that seen elsewhere in the brain.

Given that CA can result from a variety of deficits (visuo-perceptual, visuospatial short-term memory, mental imagery, motor translation and execution), it seems obvious that lesions in different sites—causing a functional damage in different loci along the processing system can be associated with CA.

A 3-year follow-up is reported, showing in the third year, an initial involvement of verbal abilities. A brief discussion of the clinical aspects of the disease is also given, together with an attempt to clarify the neural substrates of it.

Section snippets

Case report

PC was a 52-year-old right-handed woman with 8 years of education, who, in 1998, when she owned a sport shop, started to find difficulty in hanging up suits and in giving change. This last problem was due to a difficulty in grasping the money. In that period, getting dressed also became difficult: for example, she was no longer able to tie her shoelaces. She also misreached when opening the doors. Progressively, she realised that she was unable to cut cakes, to properly dress the table and to

Visuo-perceptual abilities

  • Visual exploration [1]: (i) The patient had to count all the dots on three different sheets of paper, containing, respectively 14, 34 and 36 dots. PC scored, respectively 11, 13 and 21. Omissions occurred in different positions, even centrally, and were not due to hemispatial neglect. (ii) In a second task, the patient was presented with three sheets of paper, each containing eight lines of letters. The task was to count all the A, H, and F, respectively, on each sheet. Scores were: 11 out of

July–September 2000

On 10 July the patient was reassessed on a selection of tests in order to verify the progression of her disease. A further examination continued in September. The results of the two sessions are reported together. At informal examination, PC was able to describe the major events in her life and the evolution of her disease. She was still completely autonomous. Her daily activity had not changed and she was accomplishing the same tasks as before. In particular, she was still shopping alone and

General discussion

The case of a 52-year-old woman with a progressive impairment of visuo-perceptual and constructional abilities is reported. Later in the progression of the disease, a lexical-semantic deficit appeared. PC does not show the same pattern of impairment as the patients reported by Della Sala et al. [12] she identifies objects and finds her way in the streets and in her house, because she recognises buildings, furniture and other possible visual cues. For the same reason, she did not present with a

Conclusions

In conclusion, PC seems to show, at least at the beginning of the disease, an impairment of the VSSP, in particular of the process of rehearsal, the so-called inner scribe. This can explain the impairment of all tasks, such as visual judgements, spatial mental imagery and control of movements, associated with unimpaired verbal description of stimuli and preserved simple perceptive tasks. PCs CA, which involves both, copying and spontaneous drawing, could then be attributed to a functional

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by a grant from the MIUR (Cofin 2000). The author is grateful to MariaRosa Colombo for referring the patient to her and to Luigi Trojano for helpful suggestions on testing CA. She is also grateful to Sergio Della Sala for his insightful comments on a first draft of the paper and to Chris Bird for editing the English. Paolo Nichelli organised the perfusion MRI.

References (40)

  • Baddeley AD, Lieberman K. Spatial working memory. In: Nickerson RS, editor. Attention and performance, vol. VIII....
  • A.D. Baddeley et al.

    Working memory and the vividness of imagery

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

    (2000)
  • Benton AL, Hamsher K, Varney NR, Spreen O. Contribution to neuropsychological assessment. New York: Oxford University...
  • L. Bertolani et al.

    Test di memoria non-verbale di impiego diagnostico in clinica: taratura su soggetti normali

    Archivio di Neurologia, Psichiatria e Psicologia

    (1993)
  • E. Capitani et al.

    Gruppo Italiano per lo studio dell’Invecchiamento normale. Sex differences in spatial memory: a re-analysis of block tapping long-term memory according to the short-term memory level

    The Italian Journal of Neurological Sciences

    (1991)
  • Della Sala S, Logie RH. When working memory does not work: the role of working memory in neuropsychology. In: Boller F,...
  • S. Della Sala et al.

    Slowly progressive impairment of spatial exploration and visual perception

    Neurocase

    (1996)
  • J.J. Evans et al.

    Progressive prosopoagnosia associated with selective right temporal lobe atrophy

    Brain

    (1995)
  • M.J. Farah et al.

    Visual and spatial mental imagery: dissociable systems of representation

    Cognitive Psychology

    (1988)
  • Gainotti G. Constructional apraxia. In: Fredericks JAM, editor. Handbook of clinical neurology, vol. 45. Amsterdam:...
  • Cited by (19)

    • Working memory in adults who stutter using a visual N-back task

      2021, Journal of Fluency Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      Regarding verbal information processed through the phonological loop, auditory-verbal information has direct access to the phonological store whereas visual-verbal information (e.g., printed words) needs to be visually analyzed, phonologically recoded and gain access to the phonological store through articulatory rehearsal (e.g., Baddeley et al., 1998; Repovs & Baddeley, 2006; Vallar & Baddeley, 1984; Vallar & Papagno, 2002). Regarding nonverbal visual information processed by the visuospatial sketchpad, a store component and a rehearsal mechanism, analogous to the phonological loop, have been suggested (e.g., Papagno, 2002). Working memory also includes a central executive component, which is considered to be the most complex component as it is responsible for attention focus, dividing attention, switching between tasks, decision making, and providing the interface between long-term memory and all subsystems of working memory (i.e., phonological loop, visuospatial sketchpad, episodic buffer).

    • Impaired spatial processing in visual perception, imagery and art-making following parieto-occipital infarcts

      2020, Cortex
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, inspection appears impaired, with poor processing of spatial features and incoherence of the overall percept, precluding subsequent image transformation or rotation (Sack & Schuhmann, 2012). Previous research has suggested that whereas the left temporal and parietal cortices are involved in the generation of images (Levine et al., 1985; Luzzatti et al., 1998; Papagno et al., 2002; Sack et al., 2005), the right parietal cortex is thought to be responsible for the spatial comparison of such imagined content (Sack et al., 2005). As our patient suffered damage to both parietal lobes, we propose that this bilateral insult has resulted in her marked disorder in spatial imagery.

    • The factorial structure of the mini mental state examination (MMSE) in Alzheimer's disease

      2009, Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics
      Citation Excerpt :

      In the first factor, delayed recall directly expresses a learning measure, temporal orientation describes the ability to pay attention to the date and to remember it, and spatial orientation is related to topographical learning (Giannakopoulos et al., 2000). Also, it has been demonstrated that WM is implicated in both spelling (Service and Turpeinen, 2001) and arithmetical tasks (Oberauer et al., 2001; Halpern et al., 2003), so that attention/concentration expresses an indirect value of WM. Moreover, the visuospatial sketchpad seems to represent the visual component of WM, thus playing an important role in constructional praxis (Papagno, 2002). Finally, even the impairment of verbal comprehension has been related to the WM loss rather than to impairment of linguistic knowledge in AD (Bayles, 2003).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text