Elsevier

Brain and Language

Volume 89, Issue 2, May 2004, Pages 354-361
Brain and Language

The effects of motor neurone disease on language: Further evidence

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00357-2Get rights and content

Abstract

It might sound surprising that Motor Neurone Disease (MND), regarded still by many as the very example of a neurodegenerative disease affecting selectively the motor system and sparing the sensory functions as well as cognition, can have a significant influence on language. In this article we hope to demonstrate that language dysfunction is not only a pronounced and well documented symptom in some MND patients but also that the study of language in MND can address interesting theoretical questions about the representation of language and conceptual knowledge in the brain. After a brief introduction delineating clinical and pathological features of the disease we discuss the evidence available in the literature for language dysfunction in MND. We then present linguistic data from our own study of seven patients with MND/dementia/aphasia syndrome focusing on the dissociation between noun and verb processing. To illustrate the clinical, neuropsychological and linguistic aspects of MND we describe in more detail the patient E.N., a pathologically confirmed case of MND/dementia. Finally, we attempt to characterise the nature of the linguistic impairment in MND in the light of current debates about the mechanisms underlying noun/verb dissociation.

Section snippets

Language involvement in motor neurone disease: Past evidence and future perspectives

One fundamental limitation to neuropsychological lesion studies in humans is the fact that, in many traditionally investigated conditions, the type and extent of brain damage is determined primarily by the lesion location (e.g., arterial territories in stroke, the site of the injury in trauma or the site of the tumour in neoplasms), which bears often little relation to the functional architecture of the underlying brain regions. Neighbouring areas of different embryological origin, with

The Cambridge MND/aphasia series

In the years 1996–2002 we had the opportunity to examine seven patients presenting, in a fairly uniform manner, with an insidious onset of predominantly neuropsychiatric symptoms such as obsession with food, overeating, hoarding, “childlike behaviour,” irritability, disinhibition, socially inappropriate behaviour, and, in some cases, paranoid ideas and hallucinations (one of the patients, N.N., believed that her childhood boyfriend, whom she has not seen for decades, moved into her house).

E.N.—Case description

The patient E.N. (initials changed to protect privacy) was admitted to Addenbrooke’s Hospital, Cambridge with a clinical picture similar to that of other MND/dementia cases. The history of his illness began one year earlier, when his wife noticed persecutory delusions and a change in food preference (towards chocolate and peanuts). Over the following four to five months the patient became slower, his speech slurred and his verbal output extremely limited (usually “yes” and “no”). His cognitive

Discussion: Towards a better characterisation of the linguistic deficit in MND

Our findings confirm that aphasia can be an early and prominent feature of MND/dementia. Pronounced language dysfunction was documented in all seven examined patients. In all cases it preceded the development of motor symptoms and was, therefore, unlikely to be caused by them. It is equally unlikely that the language dysfunction was a result of generalised dementia: the level of language impairment was much higher than would be expected on the basis of the results on cognitive screening tests.

Acknowledgments

We thank Tina Emery and Angela O’Sullivan for their dedicated work and their support for our patients and their families in general and E.N. in particular. We are indebted to John Xuereb and the Cambridge Brain Bank for the pathological examination of the brains of MND patients, to Simon Boniface for the neurophysiological assessment, to Karalyn Patterson and Friedemann Pulvermüller for many fruitful discussions and to Marion Wilkinson and Ashley Muir for their help in editing the manuscript.

References (48)

  • T.H. Bak et al.

    Selective impairment of verb processing associated with pathological changes in Brodmann areas 44 and 45 in the motor neurone disease–dementia–aphasia syndrome

    Brain

    (2001)
  • Bishop, D. V. M. (1989). Test for the reception of grammar (2nd ed.). London: Medical Research...
  • S. Brion et al.

    L’association maladie de Pick et sclerose laterale amyotraphique

    Encephale

    (1980)
  • S.F. Cappa et al.

    Object and action naming in Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia

    Neurology

    (1998)
  • R.J. Caselli et al.

    Rapidly progressive aphasic dementia and motor neuron disease

    Annals of Neurology

    (1993)
  • J. Constantinidis

    Syndrome familial: Association de Maladie de Pick at sclerose laterale amyotrophique

    Encephale

    (1987)
  • F. Deymeer et al.

    Thalamic dementia and motor neuron disease

    Neurology

    (1989)
  • M. Doran et al.

    Rapidly progressive aphasia with bulbar motor neurone disease: A clinical and neuropsychological study

    Behavioural Neurology

    (1995)
  • A. Eisen et al.

    Amyotrophic Lateral Sclerosis: A synthesis of research and clinical practice

    (1998)
  • I. Ferrer et al.

    Dementia of frontal lobe type and motor neuron disease. A Golgi study of the frontal cortex

    Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry

    (1991)
  • V. Gentileschi et al.

    Progressive defective recognition of familiar people

    Neurocase

    (1999)
  • T. Givon

    Syntax: Functional-typological introduction

    (1984)
  • A.E. Hillis et al.

    Modality-specific deterioration in naming verbs in nonfluent primary progressive aphasia

    Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

    (2002)
  • D. Howard et al.

    Pyramids and palm trees: A test of semantic access from pictures and words

    (1992)
  • Cited by (155)

    • What modulates the Mirror Neuron System during action observation?: Multiple factors involving the action, the actor, the observer, the relationship between actor and observer, and the context

      2021, Progress in Neurobiology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Another fMRI study found that when subjects judged sequences of three static body postures, responses in primary and supplementary motor areas were greater when those sequences were natural and fluent (ABC) than when they were unnatural and nonfluent (ACB) (Orgs et al., 2016). In addition, a number of neuropsychological and TMS studies have shown that real or virtual lesions to left inferior frontal, premotor, primary motor, and/or inferior parietal regions impair performance on nonlinguistic tasks that require judgments about static action stimuli (Tranel et al., 2003; Bak and Hodges, 2004; Saygin et al., 2004; Hillis et al., 2006; Urgesi et al., 2007; Moro et al., 2008; Fazio et al., 2009; Kemmerer et al., 2012). Furthermore, a TMS study showed that, compared with static images of either a relaxed hand or a hand at the endpoint of an action, static images of a hand executing a thumb-forefinger pincer grip increased the excitability of the muscles that would perform that action; those muscles were not affected, however, by static images of nonbiological entities with (e.g., waterfalls) or without (e.g., icefalls) implied motion (Urgesi et al., 2006; for follow-up studies see Urgesi et al., 2010, and Mattiassi et al., 2014; and for contrary data see Bunday et al., 2016).

    • Methods in neuropsychology

      2021, Encyclopedia of Behavioral Neuroscience: Second Edition
    • The spectrum of language impairments in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis

      2020, Cortex
      Citation Excerpt :

      Subsequently, this has led to the inclusion of language impairments in the recently revised diagnostic criteria (Strong et al., 2017). Early investigations of language changes focused on characterising ALS patients with aphasia (Bak, O’Donovan, Xuereb, Boniface, & Hodges, 2001; Bak & Hodges, 2004). From this arose a prominent theory that proposed that a selective impairment in processing actions was a distinctive language feature of ALS (Bak, O'Donovan, Xuereb, Boniface & Hodges, 2001; Bak & Hodges, 2004; Grossman et al., 2008; Taylor et al., 2012; York et al., 2014).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text