Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 20, Issue 3, November 2003, Pages 1649-1660
NeuroImage

Regular article
Dynamic functional changes associated with cognitive skill learning of an adapted version of the Tower of London task

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.07.003Get rights and content

Abstract

In this study, we used a modified version of the Tower of London (TOL) planning task, in conjunction with positron emission tomography, to examine the neural substrates mediating cognitive skill learning. Twelve healthy, right-handed participants solved a total of 90 different TOL problems. They were scanned on four occasions during the fast learning stage as well as in a condition designed to control for internally guided movements. Practice of the TOL task resulted in a significant decrease in planning, execution, and total time taken to solve the problems. Consistent with the results of previous studies, early performance of the TOL task was associated with increased blood flow activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal, orbitofrontal, and parietal cortices on the left, as well as in the caudate nucleus, cerebellum, and premotor cortex, bilaterally. Interestingly, however, activity in the left caudate nucleus was maintained from the beginning to the end of the learning process, suggesting that this structure plays a role in this type of cognitive skill learning. In addition, correlational analyses revealed that improved performance on the TOL task was associated with a significant decrease of activity in the medial orbitofrontal and frontopolar cortices over the course of learning, areas thought to be involved in decision making, guessing, and monitoring of feedback information. In sum, the results lend further support to the idea that the learning of cognitive skills requiring planning and working memory capacities is mediated through a fronto-striatal network.

Introduction

Neuropsychological, neurophysiological, lesion, and imaging studies have demonstrated the involvement of the frontal lobes in a variety of cognitive skills including attention, inhibition, organised search, monitoring, flexibility of strategy, problem solving, working memory, and planning (e.g., Barbas, 2000a, Barbas, 2000b, Duncan and Owen, 2000, Funahashi, 2001, Fuster, 2000a, Fuster, 2000b, Koechlin et al., 1999, Levy and Goldman-Rakic, 2000, Miller, 2000, Miller and Cohen, 2001. Other studies have recently shown that the striatum, cerebellum, and related structures also play a part in these cognitive processes Andreasen et al., 1999, Cooper et al., 1991, Dagher et al., 2001, Decety et al., 1990, Desmond and Fiez, 1998, Diamond, 2000, Downes et al., 1989, Fiez et al., 1992, Fiez, 1996, Grafman et al., 1992, Kim et al., 1994, Leiner et al., 1995, Middleton and Strick, 2000a, Middleton and Strick, 2000b, Middleton and Strick, 2001, Owen et al., 1998, Rapoport et al., 2000, Wise, 1996. However, very little attention has been given to the changes in brain function associated with improvements in performance inevitably observed following repeated trials of tasks designed to measure higher cognitive functions. Failure to address these effects in imaging studies could lead to incorrect attribution of functional activations to the cognitive process investigated (Garavan et al., 2000).

Knowledge of practice effects on performance in higher cognitive tasks has come mainly from studies investigating the cognitive determinants and neural substrates of cognitive skill learning. This ability is defined as the process by which rules, procedures, and strategies, which are relevant to the performance of a task demanding mental operations, come to be combined and applied effectively following repeated practice Ouellet, 1998, Van Lehn, 1996. To date, previous studies of cognitive skill learning have used mirror reading Kassubek et al., 2001, Poldrack et al., 1998, Poldrack and Gabrieli, 2001, verb generation Andreasen et al., 1995a, Andreasen et al., 1995b, Petersen et al., 1998, Raichle et al., 1994, Raichle, 1998, puzzle solving (Roncacci et al., 1996), recognition memory (Madden et al., 1999), and artificial grammar Fletcher et al., 1999, Knowlton et al., 1992, Peigneux et al., 1999. Yet, no study has identified the brain structures and plasticity associated with the acquisition of improved planning and problem-solving abilities through practice using brain imaging techniques. We therefore used positron emission tomography (PET) to investigate the dynamic changes associated with cognitive skill learning using a modified version of the Tower of London (TOL) planning task. Performance of the TOL is thought to require successful planning, execution, monitoring, and revision of a series of actions using working memory, as well as the selection of counterintuitive moves, which relies on inhibitory processes Chochon et al., 1999, Dehaene and Changeux, 1997, Owen, 1997, Welsh et al., 1999.

Investigations in various patient populations and with sensitive brain imaging techniques have demonstrated the critical role of the prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia in solving TOL problems Baker et al., 1996, Carlin et al., 2000, Dagher et al., 1999, Dagher et al., 2001, Elliott et al., 1997, Lange et al., 1992, Morris et al., 1988, Morris et al., 1993, Owen et al., 1990, Owen et al., 1992, Owen et al., 1995, Owen et al., 1996a, Owen et al., 1998, Rezai et al., 1993, Robbins et al., 1994, Rowe et al., 2001. On one hand, previous lesion studies in humans have shown that patients with unilateral or bilateral damage to the frontal lobes are impaired on the TOL task, as they required more moves to complete the problems compared to matched control subjects Carlin et al., 2000, Owen et al., 1990, Owen et al., 1995, Shallice, 1982. Imaging studies have also corroborated the involvement of frontal lobes during planning, as activations have been observed in those regions, and particularly in areas 9 and 46 (Brodmann) of the mid-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), using versions of the TOL task Baker et al., 1996, Dagher et al., 1999, Morris et al., 1993, Owen et al., 1996b; Rezai et al., 1993, Robbins et al., 1994. On the other hand, patients with basal ganglia disorders such as Parkinson's disease (PD) are also known to demonstrate performance deficits on the TOL task Morris et al., 1988, Owen et al., 1992. These deficits seem to be related to disease severity with mild PD symptoms affecting initial planning time, and more severe symptoms affecting problem solving accuracy as well Morris et al., 1988, Owen et al., 1992. However, the exact nature of the deficits remains a matter of debate as Hodgson and colleagues (2002) have also found significant differences in the gaze patterns used by patients when solving TOL problems, and suggest that impairments on the TOL may be due to a disruption of trans-saccadic working memory or attentional control.

Some investigators have suggested that these deficits are due to pathology of the frontal lobes (e.g., Morris et al., 1988), but others have proposed that they are caused by basal ganglia dysfunction, which disrupts frontal lobe function via the cortico-striatal loops (e.g., Lange et al., 1992, Owen et al., 1998. The latter hypothesis is based in part on PET studies in healthy subjects showing that cerebral blood flow in the caudate nucleus increases with task difficulty Dagher et al., 1999, Owen et al., 1996a, Owen et al., 1998. In addition, when PD patients are compared to age-matched controls on this task, the cerebral blood flow abnormalities are found in the basal ganglia but not the prefrontal cortex Owen et al., 1998, Dagher et al., 2001.

Finally, involvement of the cerebellum in planning is supported by imaging studies of the TOL task Baker et al., 1996, Elliott et al., 1997, Rowe et al., 2001 and by at least two other lesion studies using a similar planning task, the Tower of HanoĆÆ (TOH) Fiez et al., 1992, Grafman et al., 1992. It is not clear, however, whether activations in the latter structure are associated with the cognitive components of the task or with confounding motor factors. Indeed, other investigators have proposed that the interpretation of cerebellar activations is complicated by the fact that in imaging studies, eye movements were not explicitly matched to their control condition (Baker et al., 1996), and that subjects make eye movements during mental planning (Rowe et al., 2001). On the other hand, Hodgson et al. (2000) have shown that good planners make very few eye movements on the TOL task, suggesting that cerebellar activations might not be related to eye movements per se.

Although the TOL task typically requires participants to solve several (often 12 or more) problems, this paradigm has mainly been used as a measure of cognitive performance. To study the learning effects in planning tasks, researchers have used the TOH and Tower of Toronto (TOT) (Goel and Grafman, 1995). Learning difficulties on both tasks have been shown in patients with striatal dysfunction, such as those seen in PD and Huntington's disease (HD) (Daum et al., 1995; Saint-Cyr et al., 1988; Salmon and Butters, 1995), hence supporting the notion of a fronto-striatal involvement in cognitive skill learning Daum et al., 1995, Salmon and Butters, 1995. However, the TOT and TOH were not used in this experiment because of the declarative nature of their solution. Indeed, once a participant has memorised the sequence of moves necessary to solve a problem, planning is no longer required. The TOL, on the other hand, allows for many different solutions at different levels of difficulty, thus making it impossible to rely on declarative memory to solve the problems.

The present study was designed to determine the functional neuroanatomy underlying implicit cognitive skill learning by combining [15O]H2O PET with a modified ā€œlearning versionā€ of the TOL task. Our main objective was to investigate the nature of the dynamic functional changes occurring during repeated practice on the TOL task. Significant activations in the prefrontal cortex and striatum were expected, given the established roles of these structures in planning and other forms of skill learning.

Section snippets

Subjects

Twelve healthy, right-handed men (n = 6) and women (n = 6) participated in this experiment. The subjects were between 51 and 69 years of age (mean = 56.8, SD = 5.5) and had between 11 and 20 years of education (mean = 16.5, SD = 2.4). None had any history of neurological, psychological, or cardiovascular problems. In addition, all were tested on the Modified Mini Mental State (3MS) examination (mean = 99.3, SD = 1.37) (Folstein et al., 1975) and on the Corsi Block Tapping task (mean score =

Behavioural performance

Performance indices for both experimental and control conditions are presented in Table 1 and Fig. 2, Fig. 3. Significant changes in performance between the two control conditions were observed for planning (t = 5.8, P < 0.0001), execution (t = 4.5, P = 0.001), and total time (t = 2.5, P = 0.03). This improvement is thought to reflect nonspecific learning effects and be related to a general ability in performing the task and will not be discussed further.

Repeated-measures ANOVA revealed

Behavioural results

Decreases in the planning, execution, and total time taken to solve TOL problems seen over the 10 blocks of trials are consistent with results of previous studies from our laboratory involving both young and older participants Beauchamp, 2001, Ouellet, 1998. Therefore, repeated practice on the modified TOL task, like other tower tasks such as the TOH and TOT Butters et al., 1985, Daum et al., 1995; Saint-Cyr et al., 1988), produces significant improvements in task performance, and thus

Conclusion

Our PET data support those of other studies using the TOL planning task. Early performance on this task engaged a network of neural structures including the dlPFC, parietal areas, premotor cortex, caudate nucleus, and the cerebellum. Our behavioural and functional results also highlight the importance of taking into account the level of practice in performing functional imaging studies of cognitive tasks. Indeed, practice of TOL problems over multiple trials induced significant and rapid

Acknowledgements

We thank the subjects who participated in this study, as well as the staff of the McConnell Brain Imaging Center and Kate Hanratty for her technical assistance. This work was supported by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada through a scholarship to M.B. and by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research, through an operating grant (MOP-49480) to J.D. and A.D.

References (111)

  • J. Doyon et al.

    Distinct contribution of the cortico-striatal and cortico-cerebellar systems to motor skill learning

    Neuropsychologia

    (2003)
  • J. Duncan et al.

    Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands

    Trends Neurosci.

    (2000)
  • R. Elliott et al.

    Ventromedial prefrontal cortex mediates guessing

    Neuropsychologia

    (1999)
  • J.A. Fiez

    Cerebellar contributions to cognition

    Neuron

    (1996)
  • M.F. Folstein et al.

    Mini-mental statea practical method of grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician

    J. Psychiatr. Res.

    (1975)
  • S. Funahashi

    Neuronal mechanisms of executive control by the prefrontal cortex

    Neurosci. Res.

    (2001)
  • J.M. Fuster

    Prefrontal neurons in networks of executive memory

    Brain Res. Bull.

    (2000)
  • V. Goel et al.

    Are the frontal lobes implicated in ā€œplanningā€ functions? Interpreting data from the Tower of Hanoi

    Neuropsychologia

    (1995)
  • T.L. Hodgson et al.

    Abnormal gaze strategies during problem solving in Parkinson's disease

    Neuropsychologia

    (2002)
  • J. Kassubek et al.

    Changes in cortical activation during mirror reading before and after trainingan fMRI study of procedural learning

    Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res.

    (2001)
  • F.A. Middleton et al.

    Basal ganglia and cerebellar loopsmotor and cognitive circuits

    Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev.

    (2000)
  • F.A. Middleton et al.

    Basal ganglia output and cognitionevidence from anatomical, behavioral, and clinical studies

    Brain Cogn.

    (2000)
  • E.K. Miller

    The prefrontal cortexno simple matter

    Neuroimage

    (2000)
  • R.G. Morris et al.

    Neural correlates of planning abilityfrontal lobe activation during the Tower of London test

    Neuropsychologia

    (1993)
  • A.M. Owen

    Cognitive planning in humansneuropsychological, neuroanatomical and neuropharmacological perspectives

    Prog. Neurobiol.

    (1997)
  • A.M. Owen et al.

    Planning and spatial working memory following frontal lobe lesions in man

    Neuropsychologia

    (1990)
  • A.M. Owen et al.

    Dopamine-dependent frontostriatal planning deficits in early Parkinson's disease

    Neuropsychology

    (1995)
  • M. Petrides et al.

    Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man

    Neuropsychologia

    (1982)
  • J.B. Rowe et al.

    Imaging the mental components of a planning task

    Neuropsychologia

    (2001)
  • N.C. Andreasen et al.

    The cerebellum plays a role in conscious episodic memory retrieval

    Hum. Brain Mapp.

    (1999)
  • H. Barbas

    Complementary roles of prefrontal cortical regions in cognition, memory, and emotion in primates

    Adv. Neurol.

    (2000)
  • M.H. Beauchamp

    Changements de flux sanguin cĆ©rĆ©braux associĆ©s Ć  l'apprentissage d'une habiletĆ© cognitive par l'entremise de la tĆ¢che de la Tour de Londres

    (2001)
  • A. Bechara et al.

    Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2000)
  • A. Bechara et al.

    Different contributions of the human amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex to decision-making

    J. Neurosci.

    (1999)
  • K.S. Bordens et al.

    Research Design and MethodsA Process Approach

    (1996)
  • R. Cabeza et al.

    Imaging cognition IIan empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies

    J. Cogn. Neurosci.

    (2000)
  • F. Chochon et al.

    Differential contributions of the left and right inferior parietal lobules to number processing

    J Cogn. Neurosci.

    (1999)
  • J.A. Cooper et al.

    Cognitive impairment in early, untreated Parkinson's disease and its relationship to motor disability

    Brain

    (1991)
  • P.M. Corsi

    Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain

    Diss. Abst. Int.

    (1972)
  • A. Dagher et al.

    Mapping the network for planninga correlational PET activation study with the Tower of London task

    Brain

    (1999)
  • A. Dagher et al.

    The role of the striatum and hippocampus in planninga PET activation study in Parkinson's disease

    Brain

    (2001)
  • A.R. Damasio

    Descartes' error and the future of human life

    Sci. Am.

    (1994)
  • S. Dehaene et al.

    A hierarchical neuronal network for planning behavior

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA

    (1997)
  • M.P. Deiber et al.

    Cerebral structures participating in motor preparation in humansa positron emission tomography study

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (1996)
  • M.P. Deiber et al.

    Cortical areas and the selection of movementa study with positron emission tomography

    Exp. Brain Res.

    (1991)
  • A. Diamond

    Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex

    Child Dev.

    (2000)
  • J. Doyon et al.

    Functional anatomy of visuomotor skill learning in human subjects examined with positron emission tomography

    Eur. J. Neurosci.

    (1996)
  • J. Doyon et al.

    Functional anatomy of motor skill learning

  • R. Elliott et al.

    Prefrontal dysfunction in depressed patients performing a complex planning taska study using positron emission tomography

    Psychol. Med.

    (1997)
  • A.C. Evans et al.

    Anatomical mapping of functional activation in stereotactic coordinate space

    NeuroImage

    (1992)
  • Cited by (129)

    • Executive control training does not generalize, even when associated with plastic changes in domain-general prefrontal areas

      2019, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      Our finding could likewise be accounted for by an automatization of the inhibition process in the Complex but not Simple training group: because of the very simple rules of the simple task, it would have been already automatized at the end of the 50 min practice of the pretraining session (Chavan et al., 2015; Hartmann et al., 2015; Manuel et al., 2013). Such a decrease in prefrontal involvement with executive training has been regularly observed in studies on planning (Beauchamp et al., 2003), working memory (Hempel et al., 2004) or switching (Jimura et al., 2014) tasks. For example, diminished N2 amplitude along with a reduced RT interference effect have been associated with improved interference resolution mechanisms, assuming a shift from controlled to automatic response modes (Millner et al., 2012).

    • The effects of dynamic workload and experience on commercially available EEG cognitive state metrics in a high-fidelity air traffic control environment

      2019, Applied Ergonomics
      Citation Excerpt :

      For instance, Ayaz et al. (2012) reported decreased frontal lobe hemodynamic responses over the course of more practice sessions with an unmanned aerial vehicle task. Similar deactivation patterns have been found while participants repeatedly perform simple laboratory-based tasks (e.g., Beauchamp et al., 2003; McKendrick et al., 2014). EEG variables have also been shown to differentiate between novice and experts.

    • Trade-off of cerebello-cortical and cortico-cortical functional networks for planning in 6-year-old children

      2018, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      Likewise in adults, damage to the cerebellum, specifically the lateral regions, showed deficiency in planning skills (Grafman et al., 1992; Schmahmann, 2004). Moreover, adults showed functional activations in the prefrontal, parietal, striatal and cerebellar regions during early performance of the Tower of London (Beauchamp et al., 2003). Additionally, the cerebello-cortical system features distinct reciprocal connections of the lateral cerebellum with the prefrontal and premotor cortex, and are respectively engaged in the adjustment of contextual changes and the control of movements (Imamizu and Kawato, 2012).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text