Article Text

Download PDFPDF

A comparison of neuropsychiatric and cognitive profiles in delirium, dementia, comorbid delirium-dementia and cognitively intact controls
  1. David J Meagher1,2,
  2. Maeve Leonard1,
  3. Sinead Donnelly3,
  4. Marion Conroy3,
  5. Jean Saunders4,
  6. Paula T Trzepacz5
  1. 1Department of Adult Psychiatry, Midwestern Regional Hospital, and University of Limerick Medical School, Limerick, Ireland
  2. 2Health Systems Research Centre, University of Limerick, Limerick, Ireland
  3. 3Milford Hospice Palliative Care Centre, Limerick, Ireland
  4. 4Statistical Consulting Unit, University of Limerick, Limerick, Ireland
  5. 5Lilly Research Laboratories, Indianapolis, Indiana, USA
  1. Correspondence to Professor David Meagher, University of Limerick Medical School, Limerick, Ireland; david.meagher{at}ul.ie

Abstract

Purpose Delirium and dementia have overlapping features that complicate differential diagnosis. Delirium symptoms overshadow dementia symptoms when they co-occur, but delirium phenomenology in comorbid cases has not been compared to both conditions alone.

Methods Consecutive adults with DSM-IV delirium, dementia, comorbid delirium-dementia and cognitively intact controls were assessed using the Revised Delirium Rating Scale (DRS-R98) and Cognitive Test for Delirium (CTD).

Results Delirium and comorbid delirium-dementia groups had comparable DRS-R98 and CTD total scores, which were greater than in dementia or control groups. On the DRS-R98, multiple non-cognitive symptoms, inattention and disorientation were more severe in delirium groups compared with dementia-alone. Patients with dementia differed from both delirium groups on the CTD test of attention. Spatial span backwards was significantly lower in all patients with cognitive impairment (delirium, comorbid delirium-dementia, dementia alone) compared to controls, whereas spatial span forwards distinguished delirium groups from dementia.

Conclusions Delirium phenomenology is similar with or without comorbid dementia. A wide range of neuropsychiatric symptoms distinguish delirium from dementia. Spatial span forward is disproportionately diminished in delirium suggesting usefulness as a differentiating screening test.

  • Delirium
  • dementia
  • phenomenology
  • assessment
  • diagnosis
  • cognition

Statistics from Altmetric.com

Request Permissions

If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.

Introduction

Delirium and dementia are the two major generalised cognitive disorders that historically have been distinguished by features such as temporal course and reversibility, with delirium considered more acute in onset and fluctuating in daily symptom severity while dementias associated with a deteriorating course. The cardinal cognitive disturbance in delirium is inattention while in most dementias there is disproportionate memory disturbance with relatively preserved attention. Distinction of these disorders is important because the urgency of investigation and treatment is greater for delirium, which can reflect a medical emergency. However, some dementias have a more acute onset (eg, dementia of Lewy body type, large vascular dementia-related dementia) and distinction is further complicated by the high comorbidity such that dementia co-occurs in as many as two-thirds of delirium cases in elderly populations.1 Moreover, persistent cognitive impairments have been described during longer term follow-up of elderly patients experiencing delirium, which raises questions about reversibility and prognosis after a delirium episode.2

While acute onset, fluctuating course and attribution to an identifiable temporally related aetiology are useful distinguishing features for delirium diagnosis, there are surprisingly few studies that have compared delirium phenomenology between delirium and dementia. Most compared delirium symptoms between delirium and comorbid delirium-dementia groups, but without a ‘pure’ dementia group.3–7 Moreover, there has been limited study comparing cognitive profiles in these disorders. Floor effects for neuropsychological tests also make it challenging to find instruments useful in delirium where the level of cognitive impairment is quite severe. Two specific and validated tools have allowed for more detailed study of delirium: the Cognitive Test for Delirium (CTD)8 that measures five cognitive domains using standard neuropsychological methods and the Delirium Rating Scale-Revised-98 (DRS-R98)9 that measures a broad range of delirium symptoms not measured by other delirium instruments, including language, thought process, visuospatial ability, and both short and long-term memory.

We report a study in a palliative care setting comparing the severity of delirium symptoms in non-demented control patients to those with delirium, dementia and comorbid delirium-dementia using the DRS-R98 and the CTD. In particular, we aimed to address (1) how neuropsychiatric and cognitive profile in comorbid delirium-dementia compare to that of either disorder alone when analysed in conjunction with controls in the same setting and (2) which features best differentiate controls from delirium or dementia, and delirium groups from dementia.

Methods

Subjects and design

We conducted a prospective cross-sectional study of delirium symptoms and cognitive performance in consecutive adult cases of delirium, dementia, comorbid delirium-dementia and cognitively normal comparison subjects receiving care in the same palliative care inpatient service. Cases with altered mental state were identified on daily rounds by the palliative care medical team and consecutively referred for delirium diagnosis according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) IV criteria by the research team. Assessments were conducted by trained raters in the use of the DRS-R98 and CTD (ML or DM) and to further enhance inter-rater reliability, difficult ratings were discussed and rated by consensus between both raters. Patients who had normal cognition as determined by an Abbreviated Mental Test10 score greater than 6 points and no prior history of cognitive disturbance were randomly recruited for assessment. Dementia was defined as the presence of persistent cognitive impairment for at least 6 months prior to the assessment and per DSM criteria based on all available information at the time of assessment, including clinical case notes and collateral history from family and/or carers.11 Comorbid delirium-dementia was defined as the presence of both disorders. Each case was then assessed by first completing the DRS-R98 followed by administration of the CTD. The DRS-R98 rated the preceding 24 h period whereas the CTD measured cognition at the time of its administration. CTD responses were not used to rate DRS-R98 items. Both the DRS-R98 and the CTD are well-validated instruments, highly structured and anchored for rating and scoring.

Informed consent

The procedures and rationale for the study were explained to all patients but because many patients had cognitive impairment at entry into the study it was presumed that most were not capable of giving informed written consent. Because of the non-invasive nature of the study ethics committee approval was given to augment patient assent with proxy consent from next of kin (where possible) or a responsible care giver for all participants in accordance with the Helsinki Guidelines for Medical research involving human subjects.12

Assessments

Demographic data, medical diagnoses and medication at the time of the assessment were recorded. All available information from medical records and, where possible, collateral history was used. Nursing staff were interviewed to assist rating of symptoms over the previous 24 h.

The DRS-R989 is designed for broad phenomenological assessment of delirium. It is a 16-item scale with 13 severity and 3 diagnostic items with high inter-rater reliability, sensitivity and specificity for detecting delirium in mixed neuropsychiatric and other hospital populations.9 Each item is rated 0 (absent/normal) to 3 (severe impairment) with descriptions anchoring each severity level. Severity scale scores range from 0–39 with higher scores indicating more severe delirium. Delirium typically involves scores above 15 points (severity scale) or 18 points (total scale) when dementia is in the differential diagnosis. For determination of item frequencies in this study, any item score ≥1 was considered as being ‘present’. DRS-R98 items can be divided into cognitive (#9–13) and non-cognitive (#1–8) subscales based on construct validity.

The CTD8 was specifically designed to assess delirium patients admitted to hospital, in particular those who are intubated or unable to speak or write. It assesses five neuropsychological domains (orientation, attention, memory, comprehension and vigilance) emphasising non-verbal (visual and auditory) modalities. Tests are components of standardised and widely used neuropsychological tests. Attention on the CTD is assessed visually using the spatial span test (forwards and backwards) from the Wechsler Memory Scale.13 Each individual domain is scored from 0–6 by 2 point increments except for comprehension (single point increments). Total scores range between 0 and 30 with higher scores indicating better cognitive function and scores of less than 19 consistent with delirium. It reliably differentiates delirium from other neuropsychiatric conditions, including dementia, schizophrenia and depression.13

Statistical analyses

Statistical analysis was conducted using the SPSS v14.0 package. Demographic and rating scale data were expressed as means plus SD. Continuous variables (eg, age, total DRS-R98 and CTD scores) were compared by one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with independent t tests used for post hoc comparisons. Non-normal data (eg, DRS-R98 and CTD item scores) were compared with Mann–Whitney U tests for between group comparisons with a Bonferroni correction level of p<0.005 applied for the DRS-R98 item comparisons. Correlations between DRS-R98 and CTD item scores were made using Spearman's rank correlation coefficient.

Results

Demographic and medication data for patients from the four groups are shown in table 1. Both groups with evidence of dementia were significantly older (p<0.001) than the delirium and cognitively intact control groups. The principal causes of delirium (n=80) were systemic infection (29), metabolic disturbance (26) and drug intoxication (17), while 11 subjects from the delirium groups had a documented central nervous system (CNS) neoplastic lesion. The dementia had been diagnosed in 10 cases (3 with Alzheimer's dementia, 2 with vascular dementia, 5 with unspecified type) and was newly documented in a further 10 cases. Two patients from the dementia group had a documented CNS neoplastic lesion. The overall number of medications used was similar for the four groups but when analysis was restricted to psychotropic agents, the delirium groups both had more than the other groups. This principally reflected greater use of antipsychotic agents in delirium (67%) and delirium-dementia (58%) groups versus dementia (22%) and controls (30%) (p=0.002).

Table 1

Demographic and medication data for the patient groups (mean±SD)

Table 2 compares mean scores for delirium groups versus dementia and control groups for the DRS-R98 total and severity scales, DRS-R98 cognitive and non-cognitive subscales, and total CTD. Controls were significantly less impaired on all scores than any other group, scoring in normal ranges (p<0.001). Comorbid delirium-dementia was not significantly different from delirium except that the non-cognitive subscale was higher in delirium (p=0.04). Comorbid delirium-dementia differed from dementia on all DRS-R98 and CTD scores. Total CTD score showed only a trend (p=0.07) for delirium versus dementia. Only two in the dementia group (10%) had DRS-R98 total scores above 15 while six had total CTD scores less than 19. Figure 1 compares median total DRS-R98 scores for the four groups. Scores were higher for both delirium groups compared with dementia (p<0.001) and greater for dementia compared with controls (p<0.001). Scores did not differ between delirium groups.

Table 2

Comparison of means±SD on DRS-R98 scale scores and total Cognitive Test for Delirium (CTD) score in four groups

Figure 1

Boxplots of distribution of total DRS-R98 scores for diagnostic groups.

Mean scores for DRS-R98 items are described in table 3. Only language was similar across all groups. Both delirium and delirium-dementia groups had higher scores for the majority of symptoms when compared with dementia alone and were comparable to each other except for thought process abnormality that was worse in delirium than delirium-dementia. A wide range of DRS-R98 non-cognitive items (sleep-wake cycle, perceptual abnormality, affective lability, thought process abnormality, motor agitation and motor retardation) were more severe in both delirium groups as compared to dementia, but only thought process abnormalities and motor agitation remained statistically significant after correction for multiple testing. The only cognitive items that distinguished these groups were attention and orientation but in both cases this did not reach statistical significance after correction for multiple testing. Delirium diagnostic items (symptom fluctuation, acute onset and attributable physical disorder) significantly distinguished delirium groups from the other groups but did not distinguish dementia from controls. The dementia group differed from the control group only on the five cognitive symptoms.

Table 3

DRS-R98 item severities (mean scores±SD) for the four groups; significance is for comparisons using Mann–Whitney U test

Table 4 shows the comparison of individual CTD item scores between the four groups. Controls performed in the normal range and were significantly less impaired than any other group on each item (p<0.001) except orientation. No item distinguished comorbid delirium-dementia from delirium and only attention distinguished delirium-dementia from dementia (p<0.05). However, both attention and vigilance distinguished delirium from dementia (p<0.05). Dementia differed from controls on all five items of the CTD at p<0.001.

Table 4

Cognitive Test for Delirium (CTD) item scores for the four groups using Mann–Whitney–U test. Controls performed in the normal range for each item

Closer examination of the components of the CTD attention item—spatial span forward (SSF) and backward (SSB) measured in a visual modality—revealed that mean SSF in controls (5.7±1.6), dementia (4.1±2.1), delirium (2.6±1.9) and delirium dementia (2.8±2.3) was significantly worse for delirium versus dementia (p=0.02) and for comorbid delirium-dementia versus dementia (p=0.05) but did not differ between delirium groups. Mean SSB did not distinguish the three cognitively impaired groups from each other (dementia 2.1±1.7; delirium 1.3±1.6; and delirium-dementia 1.3±1.7) but readily distinguished all three groups from controls (mean 4.0±1.5) at p<0.001. Normal performance on SSF is 7±2 and on SSB is typically 5+2 points.14 Additionally, median scores differed significantly in a similar fashion, where distributions (see figure 2) of the middle two quartiles overlapped only partially for dementia and the comorbid group for SSF whereas there was no overlap for SSB across the three cognitively impaired groups.

Figure 2

Boxplots of distribution of scores on the Cognitive Test for Delirium (CTD) spatial span item forwards (SSF) and backwards (SSB) for diagnostic groups.

Only 1 person (2.5%) in the cognitively intact control group and 3 (15%) in the dementia group scored less than 4 points on the SSF, while 26 (65%) of the delirium group and 25 (62%) of the comorbid delirium-dementia group scored 3 or less. Using a cut-off score of less than 4 on the SSF to indicate delirium, within the three cognitively impaired groups positive predictive value was 95% and negative predictive value 35%. This suggests that subjects that score 3 or less on the SSF carry a high likelihood of having delirium (95%) but that higher scores are less useful for ruling out the presence of delirium.

The relation between perceptual disturbances/hallucinations, visuospatial function and inattention in the cognitively impaired groups was compared with correlational analysis of DRS-R98 item 2 (perceptual disturbances and hallucinations), item 10 (inattention) and item 13 (visuospatial function) as well as the SSF and SSB scores from the CTD. This indicated no significant relation between perceptual disturbances and any of the measures of visuospatial function or attention in any of the cognitive groups (rs all<0.15). In contrast, scores for visuospatial function correlated significantly with SSF (rs=0.53; p<0.001) and SSB (rs=0.46; p<0.001) for the delirium groups but not for the dementia-only group.

Discussion

In contrast to previous studies, we compared delirium phenomenology in comorbid delirium-dementia to that of both delirium and dementia groups, with a control group in the same medical setting.3–7 15 Further, our study used two well-validated instruments for delirium symptom severity—the DRS-R98 and CTD—which allow for more detailed investigation of cognitive and neuropsychiatric profile in this complex syndrome.

The need to distinguish delirium and dementia is emphasised by the greater urgency of diagnosis for delirium, which can be the first indication of serious medical morbidity16 and where late or non-detection is associated with markedly poorer outcomes including elevated mortality rates.17 Treatment response to antipsychotics is superior to use in dementia18 where use for dementia agitation and psychosis is associated with increased mortality.19 These concerns highlight the need to carefully evaluate features, including phenomenological profile, in order to better distinguish these disorders20 with revised diagnostic criteria in DSM-V and International Classification of Disease (ICD)-11 more accurately reflecting those differences. Further, comorbid delirium and dementia is understudied regarding its phenomenology and other implications, though evidence to date suggests that it is more similar to delirium. Some reports of persistent cognitive impairment in elderly delirious patients may reflect progression of previously undiagnosed dementia with poorer longer-term prognosis than in uncomplicated delirium episodes. This notion is supported by a range of neuropsychological studies21 and is consistent with longitudinal work showing an increased delirium risk in patients who have executive cognitive impairment upon admission, which reflects prehospitalisation baseline cognitive status.22

Our data using both the DRS-R98 and CTD confirmed that the domain of attentional deficits is the key distinguishing element of delirium as is represented in ICD23 and DSM11 diagnostic criteria where it is the cardinal and required symptom. We also found significantly higher scores in delirium groups on DRS-R98 items for acute onset, fluctuating course and attribution to a physical disorder. These features are well-represented in diagnostic criteria for delirium in DSM-IV and ICD-10. Further, we found evidence that supports the recently proposed three core domains of delirium—inattention (accompanied by other cognitive deficits), circadian activity disruption (sleep-wake cycle disturbance and motor activity alterations) and impaired higher level thinking ability24—being specific to delirium because items were more impaired in both delirium groups as compared to dementia. These core domain phenomenological features may be useful clinically in distinguishing delirium from dementia and should be considered for inclusion in revised ICD and DSM diagnostic criteria descriptions because current editions provide little or no guidance as to distinguishing features between delirium and dementia other than temporal course.

This study also supports previous work that comorbid delirium-dementia is virtually indistinguishable from delirium alone but that it can be distinguished from dementia on a number of features, especially non-cognitive ones. Altered motor activity, affective lability and thought process abnormalities emerged as particularly useful in distinguishing both delirium groups from dementia, while severity of thought process abnormalities was the only item that also distinguished delirium from comorbid delirium-dementia. These findings are largely consistent with those of Trzepacz et al25 where DRS-R98 items for sleep-wake cycle disturbance, thought process abnormality, motor agitation, perceptual disturbances, affective lability, attention, visuospatial ability, acute onset of symptoms, fluctuation of symptoms and physical disorder were significantly worse in delirium versus dementia, though their samples were smaller. Our findings contrast with the work of Cole et al15 regarding thought process and motor agitation levels in dementia versus delirium, but they used instruments that detect only the presence or absence of delirium symptoms and not severity as the DRS-R98 does. Further, their work found no impairment of consciousness in hyperalert delirium subgroups but did in the hypoalert, which is disconcerting because delirium is by definition a disorder of impaired consciousness. This also begs the question of what symptoms constitute impaired consciousness (sometimes called ‘clouding’) and how it is defined because this is an essential difference between delirium and dementia.

The CTD attention item, consistent with the DRS-R98 findings, best differentiates delirium groups from control and dementia groups. Further, the SSF component of this item differentiated dementia from delirium groups whereas the SSB did not. SSF is a more specific test of simple attention involving primarily sequential processing while SSB requires greater processing of information, working memory, planning ability and sequential processing26–28 with higher demands for exceptional levels of attention and concentration.29 Our data (eg, positive predictive value of 95% for a score of <4) suggest that SSF may be a useful bedside test that is relatively simple and specific to help distinguish delirium from dementia but that it is less suited to ruling out delirium due to a relatively high false-negative rate. Confirmatory work is needed.

Brown et al30 identified significantly poorer performance in patients with delirium versus Alzheimer's dementia on a range of tests of visual perception while delirious patients performed better on tests of memory. This work raises the question as to the extent to which deficits in these tests are related to impairments of visual processing (eg, in the occipital cortex) or attentional systems that direct processing towards visual inputs. Some work31 has indicated a correlation between perceptual abnormalities/hallucinations and impairment of visual perception and attention in patients with dementia but the relation between perceptual disturbances, visuospatial function and visual attention in different cognitive disorders remains unclear. Previous work using the Clock Drawing Test has indicated that it is useful for identifying patients with cognitive impairment but lacks specificity for delirium versus dementia.32 Our work suggests that differences between delirium and dementia are more evident in tests of visual attention than tests of visuospatial function. Moreover, we did not identify a major relationship between perceptual abnormalities/hallucinations and either visual attention or visuospatial performance. Further work involving patients with different dementia types are needed.

Non-cognitive delirium symptoms failed to distinguish dementia patients from medically ill controls suggesting that the presence of non-cognitive symptoms should alert clinicians to the possibility of delirium. Because we did not measure non-cognitive symptoms with a dementia-specific scale like the Neuropsychiatric Inventory33 that captures characteristic details of dementia phenomenology, we cannot be certain to what extent this would hold true for demented patients who are not admitted to hospital because neuropsychiatric symptoms are also common in dementia.34 However, there are particular characteristics of non-cognitive delirium symptoms that distinguish delirium from dementia patients, such as type of sleep-wake cycle and perceptual disturbances, extreme and rapid nature of affective lability and degree of thought process abnormality, that are captured differently on the DRS-R98 than on the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI). Additionally, we did not evaluate the stage or type of dementia, which could affect the presentation of non-cognitive symptoms. While future work should evaluate dementia more carefully to tease apart these more subtle features for differential diagnosis, it remains that delirium symptoms overshadow dementia when comorbid and that delirium, not dementia, is the medical priority not to be missed.

Unfortunately, differentiating symptoms other than inattention and temporal course are not emphasised in DSM-IV and ICD-10 delirium definitions, whereas disorganised thinking and sleep-wake cycle disturbance were emphasised in the DSM-III-R. Our data support their reinclusion in DSM-V. Providing better guidance regarding the distinction of delirium from dementia—and dementia from delirium—is a key challenge for future definitions of delirium in DSM-V20 and ICD-11.35

Study limitations

Cross-sectional studies cannot fully capture the phenomenological profile of conditions such as delirium whose symptom severity fluctuates, although the DRS-R98 uses a 24-h reporting period. In keeping with the routine assessments conducted on all admissions to the unit, the cognitively intact group was identified according to scores on the abbreviated mental test rather than a more standard test, such as the mini-mental state examination,36 which would more accurately rule out the presence of cognitive impairment typical of delirium and/or dementia. Dementia was diagnosed according to the presence of prior cognitive impairment of at least 6 months' duration. As such, a more accurate diagnosis might be obtained by using more specific criteria or an instrument such as the The Informant Questionnaire on Cognitice Decline in the Elderly (IQCODE).37 We could not specify the primary cause of dementia (ie, whether dementia was due to a degenerative process, vascular lesions, frontal dementia, alcohol, brain metastases, etc). There were fewer patients included in the dementia group perhaps reflecting the restrictions of our criteria, but also a function of the study setting where dementia is not as prevalent as in elderly medicine settings. Patients in the two groups with dementia were significantly older, which might have impacted upon the performance in cognitive tests in particular but it is relevant that the principal finding in relation to cognition indicated a superior performance in the older patients with dementia-alone as well as significant differences with both the delirium-alone group and the comorbid delirium-dementia group, which are not likely to relate to any age-associated effects. In addition, the different patterns of medication use, particularly in relation to antipsychotic agents, may have impacted our findings and warrant more detailed analysis in future work. We used only delirium instruments to compare groups and future work should also include use of the NPI,33 which measures dementia neuropsychiatric phenomenology. However, many of the NPI items are not descriptive of delirium and the ADAS-Cog38 has not been reported in delirium heretofore and may have floor effects whereby delirious patients of all severities score equally poorly due to the relative difficulty of the test. The degree to which observations in a palliative care service (where patients have terminal illness and are often in receipt of polypharmacy) can be generalised to patients in other settings is uncertain. However, our data were remarkably similar to those of Trzepacz et al24 from other medical settings. Future work in other populations where delirium and various types of dementia are common, incorporating tools designed to assess neuropsychiatric profile in dementia, can address these shortcomings and further improve our understanding of the interface between these important and highly prevalent conditions.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to acknowledge the support and co-operation of nursing and medical staff at Milford Hospice Palliative care centre.

References

View Abstract

Footnotes

  • Linked articles 205864

  • Funding This study was supported through departmental funds at Limerick Mental Health Services.

  • Competing interests Unrelated to this research, DJ Meagher has received research grant funding from Astra-Zeneca and has acted as an advisory consultant for Pfizer, Eli-Lilly, Bristol-Meyers and Janssen Pharmaceuticals, as well as receiving travel support from Smith-Kline Beecham, Eli-Lilly, Astra-Zeneca, Novartis, Wyeth and Bristol-Meyers Pharmaceuticals Ltd. T Trzepacz is a full-time salaried employee of Eli Lilly and Company and shareholder of Lilly. Lilly does not have a product with an indication for delirium. P Trzepacz owns the copyright for the Delirium Rating Scale-Revised-98 but does not charge a fee for its use except in for-profit situations.

  • Patient consent Obtained.

  • Ethics approval This study was conducted with the approval of the Midwestern Regional Hospital Ethics Committee, Dooradoyle, Limerick, Ireland.

  • Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.

Linked Articles

  • Editorial commentary
    Michael Philpot