Article Text

Divining dementia
Free
  1. J WARNER
  1. Imperial College School of Medicine, Paterson Centre, 20 South Wharf Road, London W2 1PD, UK

    Statistics from Altmetric.com

    Request Permissions

    If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.

    By the time you reach your 65th birthday, you will have over a 5% chance of having dementia.1 Or will you? The prevalence rates of dementia as 5% of the over 65s and 20% of the over 80s are often quoted, but these raw figures belie a more complex interrelation between the risk of developing dementia and the individual person. This issue of the Journal includes two articles that tackle the problem of predicting dementia. One, by Helmeret al (pp 303-309)2investigates the relation between dementia and premorbid occupation. The second, by Waite et al (pp296-302),3 explores the utility of preclinical syndromes that may presage dementia. These issues pose ethical and social problems, but may be important in improving the care of patients with dementia

    The suggestion that high premorbid intellect protects against the development of dementia has been elegantly demonstrated in the Nun study, which found that individuals who wrote grammatically complex sentences and demonstrated high idea density (average number of ideas per 10 words) had better cognitive function and lower rates of dementia six decades later.4 Whether premorbid intellect is negatively correlated with later cognitive impairment because increased neuronal branching and connectivity related to learning are protective, or because individuals just have further to fall, remains unclear.

    Although Helmer et al reported that female farmers seemed to be at greater risk of developing dementia with parkinsonism, occupation was a poor predictor of risk for developing Alzheimer's disease. At first this seems counterintuitive, as cognitive functioning and occupation are closely linked. However, other factors may play a part in later development of dementia—for example, lower ongoing intellectual stimulation (the use it or lose it paradigm) and lifestyle and occupational factors that may be implicated in the aetiology of dementia. Some occupational risk factors such as repeated head injury have been identified although others (Helmeret al 2 suggest pesticide use among farmers) remain speculative.

    Preclincial syndromes that indicate high risk of developing dementia may be important to recognise. Use of cholinesterase inhibitors such as donepezil, rivastigmine or galantamine in those with subclinical dementia may lead to better treatment success than is currently the case when treating established disease. Although recently approved by the National Institute for Clinical Excellence in the United Kingdom, cholinesterase inhibitors result in only marginal improvements in established mild to moderate Alzheimer's disease. For example, you would need to treat 17 people with rivastigmine rather than placebo for one additional patient to have a 4 point improvement on the 70 point ADAS cognitive assessment scale after 26 weeks.5

    Other benefits may also result from early diagnosis: the ability to plan for your illness (for example, writing a will, advance directive, and an enduring power of attorney while capacity exists), taking the trip of a lifetime, talking to estranged relatives and friends, and fulfilling other longstanding ambitions. Early diagnosis may also help to remove the atmosphere of secrecy and stigma the label still confers.

    Waite et al 4 identify a problem with the current role of preclinical syndromes in predicting dementia; the relatively low positive predictive value. The current answer to the question “if I have a preclinical syndrome what are the chances of me developing dementia in the next 3 years?” is “around 20%”. This leads to an ethical conundrum; whether or not to screen people for preclinical syndromes, as doing so should mean having to inform them of the result despite the high false positive rate. It is essential to minimise false positives when diagnosing dementia because of the significance of the diagnosis and, as Waite et al 3suggest, screening currently remains a research tool.

    The need to identify accurately people with early or preclinical dementia has never been more acute. The most important approaches to achieve this include changing illness behaviour by increasing awareness and reducing the stigma of dementia among the public, promoting early recognition and referral by general practitioners and building “risk profiles” that are predictive of dementia.

    References

    Footnotes

    • j.warner{at}ic.ac.uk

    Linked Articles