Article Text
Abstract
Background Binary reversals (exemplified by ‘yes’/‘no’ confusions) have been described in patients with primary progressive aphasia (PPA) but their diagnostic value and phenotypic correlates have not been defined.
Methods We conducted a retrospective cohort study analysing demographic, clinical, neuropsychological, linguistic and behavioural data from patients representing all major PPA syndromes (non-fluent/agrammatic variant, nfvPPA; logopenic variant, lvPPA; semantic variant, svPPA) and behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD). The prevalence of binary reversals and behavioural abnormalities, illness duration, parkinsonian features and neuropsychological test scores were compared between neurodegenerative syndromes, and the diagnostic predictive value of binary reversals was assessed using logistic regression.
Results Data were obtained for 83 patients (21 nfvPPA, 13 lvPPA, 22 svPPA, 27 bvFTD). Binary reversals occurred in all patients with nfvPPA, but significantly less frequently and later in lvPPA (54%), svPPA (9%) and bvFTD (44%). Patients with bvFTD with binary reversals had significantly more severe language (but not general executive or behavioural) deficits than those without reversals. Controlling for potentially confounding variables, binary reversals strongly predicted a diagnosis of nfvPPA over other syndromes.
Conclusions Binary reversals are a sensitive (though not specific) neurolinguistic feature of nfvPPA, and should suggest this diagnosis if present as a prominent early symptom.
- PRIMARY PROGRESSIVE APHASIA
- FRONTOTEMPORAL DEMENTIA
- DEMENTIA
This is an open access article distributed in accordance with the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 Unported (CC BY 4.0) license, which permits others to copy, redistribute, remix, transform and build upon this work for any purpose, provided the original work is properly cited, a link to the licence is given, and indication of whether changes were made. See: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
Statistics from Altmetric.com
Introduction
Diagnosis of primary progressive aphasia (PPA) is challenging even for expert clinicians.1 Given the current dearth of objective biomarkers in these diseases, clinical phenotyping remains paramount. ‘Binary reversals’—selection of the wrong alternative from a pair of candidate opposite verbal responses (most often ‘yes’/‘no’), frequently with spontaneous self-correction—have been reported as a phenomenon marring everyday communication in people with PPA.2–5 This symptom may constitute a specifically neurolinguistic feature, rather than reflecting a more generalised deficit of behaviour regulation.5 However, the diagnostic value of binary reversals has not been clarified.
Here we addressed this issue in a large, well characterised patient cohort representing all canonical syndromes of PPA and the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD). We assessed the prevalence of binary reversals in relation to syndromic diagnosis and associated clinical, neuropsychological and behavioural features. Based on clinical experience and previously published observations,2–5 we hypothesised that binary reversals would be more prevalent in the non-fluent/agrammatic variant of PPA (nfvPPA) than other syndromes and would be associated with linguistic deficits rather than behavioural abnormalities.
Methods
Assessment of patients
We assessed all patients in our active research cohort at the Dementia Research Centre who fulfilled consensus diagnostic criteria for nfvPPA, logopenic variant PPA (lvPPA), semantic variant PPA (svPPA) or bvFTD.6 7 All had syndromes of mild-to-moderate severity and supportive brain MRI with minimal cerebrovascular burden. Patient group characteristics are summarised in table 1.
Supplemental material
Using a structured clinical survey, we recorded the presence (or absence) of binary reversals and other potentially relevant behavioural symptoms following illness onset (online supplemental table S1) in online supplemental file 1), consulting with each patient’s primary caregiver or equivalent close informant; informants were invited to provide examples of the symptom. Patients underwent neurological examination and a comprehensive neuropsychological assessment (table 1). In addition, we recorded whether binary reversals were associated with parkinsonism and/or a diagnosis of corticobasal syndrome (CBS) or progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP).
Statistical analyses
Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS V.28.0 and R (V.4.3.1). Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests and Levene’s tests were first conducted to check for normality and homogeneity of variance, respectively. A one-way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) assessed for age differences and a Kruskal-Wallis test for differences in Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) scores between patient groups (irrespective of binary reversal status). The Kruskal-Wallis test was used to compare illness duration in the nfvPPA cohort versus patients with and without binary reversals in other syndromic categories. For binarised data (symptoms present/absent), group differences were assessed using χ2 tests. Neuropsychological and behavioural associations of binary reversals were assessed within the bvFTD group (the largest diagnostic group) by comparing patient subgroups with and without binary reversals using independent-samples t-tests or Mann-Whitney U tests; results from two-tailed tests are reported. A binomial logistic regression model was used to assess the likelihood of having a diagnosis of nfvPPA versus other syndromes in the presence of binary reversals, incorporating age, symptom duration and MMSE score as covariates in the model and also without these covariates. Alpha threshold 0.05 was used for all comparisons. Multiple comparison correction was not performed, given the relatively small sample size (substantial risk of failing to detect a true effect) and lack of independence of surveyed characteristics.
Results
Data from 83 patients (21 nfvPPA, 13 lvPPA, 22 svPPA, 27 bvFTD) were available for analysis (table 1). Within the nfvPPA group, two patients fulfilled criteria for primary progressive apraxia of speech8 (table 1). Patient groups did not differ significantly in age (F(3,79) = 2.08, p=0.11) or overall disease severity indexed using MMSE (χ2(3) = 7.12, p=0.07).
Binary reversals were reported across syndromic groups, although with widely varying prevalence: binary reversals were reported in all patients with nfvPPA, but significantly less frequently (p<0.05) in patients with lvPPA (54%), bvFTD (44%) and svPPA (9%). Informant descriptions indicated that reversals most commonly involved mis-selection of ‘yes/no’, but diverse other examples were produced (including ‘left/right’, ‘he/she’, ‘up/down’, ‘open/shut’, ‘good/bad’, ‘hot/cold’, ‘north/south’); non-verbal communication gestures (thumbs up/down, head nod/shake) could also be affected. The subgroups of patients with bvFTD and lvPPA who reported binary reversals had significantly longer mean symptom duration than the nfvPPA group (both p<0.05), whereas symptom duration in patients with bvFTD, lvPPA and svPPA who had no binary reversals was similar to the nfvPPA group (p>0.05).
Within the bvFTD group (table 1, figure 1), binary reversals were significantly associated with younger age at testing (t(25) = 2.85, p<0.01), longer illness duration (Mann-Whitney U=46.00, p=0.03) and more severe deficits of phonological fluency (t(24) = 2.22, p=0.04), phonological working memory (reverse digit span) (t(25) = 2.10, p=0.046), single word comprehension (Mann-Whitney U=43.50, p=0.04) and reading (National Adult Reading Test, t(24) = 3.10, p<0.01; Stroop word reading, Mann-Whitney U=126.5, p=0.03). Reversals were not significantly associated with other executive, general cognitive or behavioural deficits in the bvFTD group (all test statistics presented in full in online supplemental table S2 and online supplemental figure S1). A qualitatively similar pattern of more severe language deficits in patients exhibiting binary reversals was present in the lvPPA and svPPA groups (table 1).
Parkinsonian features were present in 62% of patients with nfvPPA (half with a diagnosis of PSP or CBS) but were less prevalent in other syndromic groups and not consistently associated with binary reversals (table 1).
After covarying for potentially confounding factors of age, illness duration and overall severity (and applying Firth’s bias reduction method9 to account for the universality of binary reversals in the nfvPPA group), the presence of binary reversals conferred significantly higher ORs for a diagnosis of nfvPPA versus all other syndromes (OR=5.07, 95% CI (2.74 to 10.04), p<0.001) versus all other PPA syndromes (OR=5.07, 95% CI (2.74 to 10.01), p<0.001) and versus individual syndromes of bvFTD (OR=5.30, 95% CI (2.46 to 10.73), p<0.001), lvPPA (OR=3.87, 95% CI (1.41 to 8.84), p<0.001) and svPPA (OR=5.39, 95% CI (2.94 to 11.25), p<0.001). Similarly significant results were obtained from the model using only binary reversals as the independent predictor (online supplemental table S3).
Discussion
We have shown that binary reversals strongly predict a diagnosis of nfvPPA, developing in a high proportion (here 100%) of patients with this syndrome and significantly more frequently than in other PPA syndromes or bvFTD. Binary reversals were uncommon in svPPA and though encountered in around half of patients with lvPPA and bvFTD, developed later and/or in the context of more severe cognitive impairment in these syndromes than in nfvPPA. This feature may therefore have higher diagnostic specificity earlier in the course of the illness.
While this study does not elucidate the pathophysiological mechanism, it is noteworthy that, within the bvFTD group, patients with binary reversals performed significantly less well on neurolinguistic measures (phonological fluency, phonological working memory, word comprehension and reading) than patients who did not make reversals, whereas the two subgroups had otherwise comparable executive, general cognitive and behavioural profiles. This suggests that the development of binary reversals can form part of the neurolinguistic phenotype of bvFTD.10 Although it was not possible to analyse the specific associations of binary reversals in the nfvPPA group (since reversals were universal in this group), no single behavioural feature nor the presence of clinical parkinsonism, PSP or CBS was required for binary reversals to manifest. Taken together, our findings suggest that binary reversals are a neurolinguistic phenomenon, rather than a non-specific consequence of executive dysregulation, in line with previous reports of similar reversals in aphasic stroke.11 On the other hand, impaired response inhibition (as indexed by impaired Stroop task performance) was present in all patient groups exhibiting binary reversals, which may signify a complex interplay of causative and permissive factors.2 3
Further work is needed to characterise the semiology of binary reversals and the circumstances that provoke them. Here we simply recorded the occurrence of the symptom; quantifying the frequency and severity of binary reversals and tracking their longitudinal development in the individual patient would give a more nuanced picture and establish how this symptom relates to other features of the illness. Anecdotally, a similar phenomenon occurs in nfvPPA patients speaking languages other than English: this requires substantiation. The neural basis of the symptom also remains to be defined. Our nfvPPA cohort is fairly typical neuropsychologically and neurologically of other published series,12 13 and (considered alongside previous observations2 3) the propensity of this syndrome to manifest binary reversals may reflect the targeting of fronto-subcortical circuitry by causative tauopathies. However, unless binary reversals have led to an important communication failure, this symptom may not be volunteered.4 We propose that clinicians suspecting PPA should seek a history of binary reversals and—particularly where early and prominent—this curious phenomenon may constitute a useful diagnostic clue. Recognition will enable investigation and management, including speech and language therapy for patients in whom binary reversals present a significant issue for communication in daily life.
Ethics statements
Patient consent for publication
Ethics approval
The study was approved by the University College London institutional ethics committee (reference no. 06/Q0512/52) and all participants gave informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to all patients and caregivers for their participation.
Supplementary materials
Supplementary Data
This web only file has been produced by the BMJ Publishing Group from an electronic file supplied by the author(s) and has not been edited for content.
Footnotes
X @charl_marshall, @cjdhardy
Contributors Conception and design of the work: EM, LC, AC, CH and JDW. Data collection, analysis and interpretation; critical revision of the work for intellectual content; final approval of the version to be published: all authors. Study supervision: CH and JDW.
Funding The Dementia Research Centre is supported by Alzheimer's Research UK, Brain Research UK and the Wolfson Foundation. This work was supported by the Alzheimer’s Society, the Royal National Institute for Deaf People, Alzheimer’s Research UK and the National Institute for Health Research University College London Hospitals Biomedical Research Centre. Individual authors were supported by an Association of British Neurologists Clinical Research Training Fellowship (to JCSJ); a UCL Graduate and Overseas Research Scholarship (to LC); Bart’s Charity (grant to CM); an NIHR Advanced Fellowship (to AV); a Miriam Marks Brain Research UK Senior Fellowship, Medical Research Council Clinician Scientist Fellowship (MR/M008525/1) and the NIHR Rare Disease Translational Research Collaboration (grant BRC149/NS/MH) (to JDR); a RNID–Dunhill Medical Trust Pauline Ashley Fellowship (grant PA23_Hardy) (to CH); and the National Brain Appeal (Frontotemporal Dementia Research Studentship in Memory of David Blechner) (JDW). This research was funded in part by UKRI. For the purpose of Open Access, the authors have applied a Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) public copyright licence to any Author Accepted Manuscript version arising from this submission. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NIHR or the Department of Health and Social Care.
Competing interests None declared.
Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.
Supplemental material This content has been supplied by the author(s). It has not been vetted by BMJ Publishing Group Limited (BMJ) and may not have been peer-reviewed. Any opinions or recommendations discussed are solely those of the author(s) and are not endorsed by BMJ. BMJ disclaims all liability and responsibility arising from any reliance placed on the content. Where the content includes any translated material, BMJ does not warrant the accuracy and reliability of the translations (including but not limited to local regulations, clinical guidelines, terminology, drug names and drug dosages), and is not responsible for any error and/or omissions arising from translation and adaptation or otherwise.