We read with great interest the article by Fukuda et al. “Facial
nerve motor-evoked potential monitoring during skull base surgery predicts
facial nerve outcome.” (J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008; 79:1066-1070).
In this rather small group of 26 patients with skull base tumors, the
authors have tried to establish a role for facial nerve motor evoked
potentials (FNMEPs) to predicting the neuro...
We read with great interest the article by Fukuda et al. “Facial
nerve motor-evoked potential monitoring during skull base surgery predicts
facial nerve outcome.” (J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008; 79:1066-1070).
In this rather small group of 26 patients with skull base tumors, the
authors have tried to establish a role for facial nerve motor evoked
potentials (FNMEPs) to predicting the neurological outcome in the facial
nerve. Motor evoked potentials were recorded from upper and lower facial
nerve’s innervated muscles after transcranial electrical stimulation
(TES).
Unfortunately, the motor response in the upper facial muscle
(orbicularis oculi, in the authors study) can result not only from the
activation of the corticobulbar (corticonuclear) pathway in the facial
nucleus, but also from the blink reflex (BR) after supraorbital nerve
(trigeminal afferents) activation. This effect very often occurs after a
strong TES due to the spreading of current over the anterior scalp, which
activates the branches of the supraorbital nerve. When a strong TES with a
short train of stimuli is used, as authors have in their study, the same
stimulation parameters are appropriate for eliciting the first component
(R1) of the BR in the orbicularis oculi muscle (Fig 1). (Deletis et al.
2008).
Therefore, if one is to use the methodology of the FNMEPs for
monitoring the upper motoneuron (corticobulbar pathway) the information
can be misleading; e.g. response at the orbicularis oculi muscle will be
present despite a lesion to the corticobulbar tract of the upper facial
muscle. In fact, the unrecognizable response may belong to the BR.
Transcranial magnetic stimulation studies (TMS) reveal, furthermore,
that the R1 component of the BR has about the same latency as FNMEPs;
therefore this latter response may be influenced by the BR (Cruccu and
Hallet 2006). Due to the fact that the upper facial nucleus have
bicortical (bi hemispheric) innervation, the patient still will not have
‘central’ facial nerve palsy on the upper face in the surgically induced
lesion at the cortex/subcortex level. Facial paresis/palsy will be evident
only if the lesion is close to the facial nucleus, where both
corticobulbar tracts originating from the right and left cortex may be
damaged.
Furthermore in infratentorial surgery, if FNMEPs are in fact the BR,
any lesion of trigeminal afferents from the upper face will result in the
intraoperative changes or disappearance of the response, while
postoperatively volitional movement of the upper face will be preserved.
One strategy to distinguish these two responses may be to
simultaneously monitor FNMEPs, through TES and BR, by stimulating the
supraorbital nerve with a short train of stimuli. As a reflex in nature,
the BR elicitation is sensitive to anesthesia and that is why it is not
elicitable in all patients (Deletis et al. 2008). In case the BR is not
elicitable by supraorbital nerve stimulation, and there is a response in
the orbicularis oculi after TES, it is highly probable that the response
has originated from the corticobulbar tract but not from the BR. So far
there is no way to figure out the origin in case that both ways of
stimulation (supraorbital nerve and TES) generate a response in the
orbicularis oculi muscle.
To conclude, the recording of FNMEPs from the orbicularis oculi
cannot be qualified solely as a cortical/subcortical origin, since it may
also be a BR. So far, no established methodology exists to distinguish
these two responses (BR and FNMEPs for upper face) in the setting of
intraoperative monitoring, especially since they have similar latencies
and behaviors.
Although Fukuda et al. use FNMEPs for monitoring the peripheral part
of the facial nerve, we believe that given the reasons above, using
FNMEPs for the upper facial muscles should be performed together with BR
monitoring.
References
1. Cruccu G, Hallet M. Brainstem function and dysfunction.
Clin
Neurophysiol. (Suppl.) 2006, Vol.58, pp 4.
2. Deletis V, Urriza J, Ulkatan S, et al. The feasibility of recording
blink reflexes under general anesthesia.
Muscle & Nerve. In press.
Multiple sclerosis is believed to be an autoimmune pathology, yet the
mechanisms triggering the disease remain elusive. Therefore, I read with
great interest the paper by Zamboni and his team who investigated the
venous hemodynamics in patients with multiple sclerosis. His findings that
this disease might be attributable to venous refluxes shed new light on
the facts that have been known for decades...
Multiple sclerosis is believed to be an autoimmune pathology, yet the
mechanisms triggering the disease remain elusive. Therefore, I read with
great interest the paper by Zamboni and his team who investigated the
venous hemodynamics in patients with multiple sclerosis. His findings that
this disease might be attributable to venous refluxes shed new light on
the facts that have been known for decades (1), but have been mostly
ignored by the scientific community. It should be answered, however, how
this pathological outflows in the extra- and intracranial veins could
trigger autoimmune reaction.
The loss of integrity of the blood-brain barrier, which is primarily
built by tight-junction complexes between adjacent endothelial cells of
the cerebrovascular endothelium, is a hallmark of multiple sclerosis. In
this context, parallels between multiple sclerosis and chronic venous
insufficiency of lower extremities (which is also an endotheliocyte-
focused pathology) could be helpful. Yet, it should be remembered that
endotheliocytes building the blood-brain barrier highly differ from those
in the periphery. Moreover, venous hypertension in cerebral and spinal
veins is unlikely to disassemble the cerebrovascular endothelial barrier
(2). Indeed, although Zamboni has found increased venous pressure distally
of venous stenoses, these pressure gradients were rather small. Thus, it
is more likely that not venous hypertension, but pathological pattern of
the blood flow-associated forces, decreased level of shear stress in
particular, disassembles the blood-brain barrier and increases the
transendothelial permeability. It has been found that an enhanced
expression of pivotal tight junction proteins: occludin and ZO-1 in the
cerebrovascular endothelium was associated with reduced transendothelial
permeability and it has been shown, moreover, that an increased shear
stress, especially with pulsatile flow characteristics, upregulated these
proteins (3). By contrast, loss of shear stress after flow cessation
enhanced the blood-brain barrier permeability (4). Therefore, a reduced
shear, for instance as a result of the refluxing venous blood flow, could
potentially result in the weakening of the blood-brain barrier. This in
turn could initiate an autoimmune attack against nervous tissue.
Several questions, though, should have been answered before multiple
sclerosis was recognized as a fundamentally hemodynamic disorder. First,
if intracranial reflux is indeed the trigger of multiple sclerosis
plaques, and not an innocent bystander. This will require additional
studies in a much larger cohort of multiple sclerosis patients. Second, if
these refluxes only bypass an occlusion and affect exclusively large
cerebral and spinal veins, or they extend also into smaller veins
(probable, damage of the blood-brain barrier can occur on condition that a
decreased shear stress influence the cerebrovascular endothelium in the
postcapillary venules). Third, mechanistic links between venous refluxes
and cellular and molecular pathways that are responsible for autoimmune
reaction should be determined, since such a discovery could result in the
development of novel effective pharmacologic agents. Regarding these
mechanisms, it should be suspected that the low shear-induced expression
of ICAM-1 (adhesion molecule, which is responsible for the firm adhesion
of leukocytes to endothelia) by cerebrovascular endothelium, could be a
critical point in the initiation of multiple sclerosis plaque, since the
crosslinking of ICAM-1 with integrins expressed on the leukocytic surfaces
leads to a weakening of the blood-brain barrier and facilitates the
transendothelial leukocyte migration (5).
Furthermore, consequently to Zamboni’s findings it may be
speculated that at least for some anatomical variants of pathological
venous outflow, surgical correction of steno-occlusions can be a
therapeutic option in addition to or instead of pharmacotherapy.
In conclusion, investigations of the extra- and intracranial
hemodynamic disturbances in multiple sclerosis patients appear to be a
challenging avenue and may open a new chapter of therapeutic approach to
this debilitating disease.
References
1. Schelling F. Damaging venous reflux into the skull or spine: relevance
to multiple sclerosis.
Med Hypothes 1986;21:141-8.
2. Simka M. Evidence against the role for dural arteriovenous fistulas in
the pathogenesis of multiple sclerosis.
Med Hypothes 2008;71:619.
3. Colgan OC, Ferguson G, Collins NT, et al. Regulation of bovine brain
microvascular endothelial tight junction assembly and barrier function by
laminar shear stress.
Am J Physiol Heart Circ Physiol 2007;292:H3190-7.
4. Krizanac-Bengez L, Mayberg MR, Cunningham E, et al. Loss of shear
stress induces leukocyte-mediated cytokine release and blood-brain barrier
failure in dynamic in vitro blood-brain barrier model.
J Cell Physiol
2006;206:68-77.
5. Lyck R, Reiss Y, Gerwin N, Greenwood J, Adamson P, Engelhardt B. T-cell
interaction with ICAM-1/ICAM-2 double-deficient brain endothelium in
vitro: the cytoplasmic tail of endothelial ICAM-1 is necessary for
transendothelial migration of T cells.
Blood 2003;102:3675-83.
There is no conflict of interest regarding this letter.
We are writing in response to the meta-analysis by Doubal, Hokke and
Wardlaw “Retinal Microvascular Abnormalities and Stroke – A Systematic
Review” published online on 17 October 2008. While the paper is well
written, the topic interesting and potentially important, we have several
concerns regarding the methodological rigor of the meta-analysis.
We are writing in response to the meta-analysis by Doubal, Hokke and
Wardlaw “Retinal Microvascular Abnormalities and Stroke – A Systematic
Review” published online on 17 October 2008. While the paper is well
written, the topic interesting and potentially important, we have several
concerns regarding the methodological rigor of the meta-analysis.
The fundamental issue is inappropriate pooling of data. An important
and basic tenet of meta-analysis is that study-specific estimates should
be rigorously assessed to determine if they can be reasonably pooled. Each
component study should measure a similar effect in a similar manner.
Sources for clinical heterogeneity between studies need to be carefully
considered and may include the study population’s characteristics, the
exposure, comparator and follow-up time, outcome of interest, and the
method used to calculate each study-specific measure of effect.
On closer inspection of the data, many of these sources of clinical
heterogeneity may have been overlooked in the pooling process in Doubal et
al. We highlight some here.
1. Inclusion of duplicate subject populations
Of the nine meta-analyses undertaken, study population duplication
potentially occurred in six (67%). Duplication is a major methodological
flaw that could lead to biased pooled estimates.
The forest plots in Figures 1 and 2, and the Results text appear to
refer to results calculated using duplicate subject populations. This is
in contrast to the section in the Methods, where the authors specifically
state that they were “careful to count patients from studies contributing
more than one paper only once to avoid unnecessary bias”.
For example, in the analysis of retinopathy and incident stroke the
authors included three effect estimates from the same ARIC study cohort.
ARIC-Wong 20011 reported on subjects without a history of stroke
(regardless of diabetes status) whilst ARIC-Wong 20022 is a subset of ARIC
cohort subjects who had MRI data and ARIC-Cheung 20073 another subset of
ARIC cohort subjects with diabetes.
2. Differing exposure, comparator and outcomes pooled together
Using effect estimates from studies with different exposures,
comparators and outcomes may not be appropriate in the meta-analysis
[Section 13.6.2.4 of 4] For instance, in the analysis of retinopathy and
incident stroke, we question whether these studies are too clinically
heterogeneous to pool. The reported pooled estimate includes exposures and
comparators such as a one-step increase on a 15-step retinopathy scale5,
moderate or worse retinopathy (compared to less than moderate
retinopathy)6, proliferative retinopathy (compared to none)7 to any
retinopathy (compared to none)1-3, 8, 9. Likewise, the pooled estimate
includes heterogeneous outcomes that were variously defined as self-
reported (and unconfirmed) nonfatal stroke5, hospitalisation for, or death
during admission from, ischemic stroke (specifically excluding other types
of stroke and deaths outside of hospital)1 and death certificate confirmed
fatal stroke7. And finally, the subject populations differed, ranging from
general population with no history of stroke1, 2, persons without diabetes
(with or without a history of CVD)6 and men with type 2 diabetes9.
3. Inclusion of variously adjusted and unadjusted estimates in same
meta-analysis
The inclusion in meta-analysis of “risk ratios” that have been
adjusted along with those that are unadjusted is potentially misleading.
The interpretation of unadjusted and adjusted effects differs [Section
13.6.2.2 of 4]. Unadjusted effect estimates, the “population average
effect”, can be interpreted as the effect in a population with an average
mixture of characteristics. In contrast, adjusted effect estimates, or
“conditional estimates”, should be interpreted as the effect of the
exposure that would be observed in groups with particular combinations of
the adjusted covariates. Additionally, research has shown that conditional
(i.e. adjusted) estimates usually show a larger effect than population
average (i.e. unadjusted) estimates [Section 13.6.2.2 of 4]; providing yet
another source of potential bias for meta-analysis.
For example, it seems inappropriate to combine an estimate of the
relative hazard of incident stroke relating to the presence of RVO after
accounting for time-to-event, age, sex, race, body mass index, current
smoking, presence of hypertension, diabetes and glaucoma10 with another
study’s assessment of
the crude numbers of strokes in a clinical population of RVO patients
(compared to a regional population rate)11?
In addition, differing combinations and definitions of covariates in
adjusted “risk ratios” may impact the ability to reasonably pool study-
specific effect estimates. For example, we query whether adjustment for
“diabetes” in a study of only non-diabetics is comparable to adjustment
for diabetes in a general population, and therefore, is it reasonable to
pool these effect estimates? On review of the component papers it seems
that many of the effect estimates are from populations defined by their
diabetic status yet are labelled spuriously in the forest plots as
“adjusted for diabetes”.
4. Follow-up time for incident events
In their Methods, Doubal et al state that since stroke outcomes were
rare, odds ratios, hazard ratios and relative risks can be consider
equivalent. However, the methods by which each type of effect estimate is
calculated differ in their adjustment for an important confounder: follow-
up time or time-to-event. Obviously, time-to-event information is
important when assessing any prospective relationship between retinal
signs and stroke.
The authors refer to their use of “risk ratios” from each study in
the analyses, however, it is unclear if these are published, or were
calculated by Doubal et al, and if these are hazard ratios, odds ratios or
relative risks. If the utilised effect estimates are hazard ratios derived
from Cox models then presumably the effect of time-to-incident-stroke has
been accounted for in their calculation. If they are odds ratios derived
from logistic models, the estimate relates to the effect seen over a fixed
study-specific period of follow-up, so follow-up time may become an
important source of clinical heterogeneity between studies (e.g. Is it
reasonable to pool effects over 5 years with effects over 20 years when
the incidence of stroke is known to increase with time but the
relationship may not be uniform?). If they are relative risks, the
inclusion of time-to-event information becomes less clear.
In conclusion, although the authors are to be congratulated on doing
a systematic review in an emerging area of interest, we have concerns
regarding the methodological rigor of the meta-analysis and thus the
resulting pooled estimates. We encourage the authors to consider
reanalysing of some of their data so that appropriate conclusions can be
drawn.
References
1. Wong TY, Klein R, Couper DJ, et al. Retinal microvascular
abnormalities and incident stroke: The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities
Study.
Lancet 2001;358(9288):1134-40.
2. Wong TY, Klein R, Sharrett AR, et al. Cerebral white matter
lesions, retinopathy, and incident clinical stroke.
Jama 2002;288(1):67-
74.
3. Cheung N, Rogers S, Couper DJ, et al.
Is diabetic retinopathy an
independent risk factor for ischemic stroke?
Stroke 2007;38(2):398-401.
4. Higgins JPT, Green S, eds. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic
Reviews of Interventions Version 5.0.1 [updated September 2008]: The
Cochrane Collaboration,
available from www.cochrane-handbook.org, 2008.
5. Klein B, Klein R, McBride P, et al. Cardiovascular disease,
mortality, and retinal microvascular characteristics in type 1 diabetes:
Wisconsin epidemiologic study of diabetic retinopathy.
Archives of
Internal Medicine 2004;164(7):1917-24.
6. Hirai FE, Moss SE, Knudtson MD, et al. Retinopathy and Survival in
a Population without Diabetes: The Beaver Dam Eye Study.
Am J Epidemiol
2007;166(6):724-30.
7. Klein R, Klein BEK, Moss SE, Cruickshanks KJ. Association of
ocular disease and mortality in a diabetic population.
Archives of
Ophthalmology 1999;117(11):1487-95.
8. Mitchell P, Wang JJ, Wong TY, et al. Retinal microvascular signs
and risk of stroke and stroke mortality.
Neurology 2005;65(7):1005-9.
9. Fuller JH, Stevens LK, Wang S-L, WHO Multinational Study Group. Risk factors for cardiovascular mortality and morbidity: The WHO
multinational study of vascular disease in diabetes.
Diabetologia
2001;44(Suppl 2):S54-S64.
10. Cugati S, Wang JJ, Knudtson MD, et al. Retinal Vein Occlusion and
Vascular Mortality. Pooled Data Analysis of 2 Population-Based Cohorts.
Ophthalmology 2007;114(3):520-4.
11. Tsaloumas MD, Kirwan J, Vinall H, et al. Nine year follow-up
study of morbidity and mortality in retinal vein occlusion.
Eye
2000;14(6):821-7.
I have read with great interest the article by Edison et al about the
amyloid load in patients with Parkinson´s disease (PD), PD with dementia
(PDD) and dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) (1). Authors hypothesised that
amyloid pathology would be uncommon in PD without dementia, an occasional
feature of PDD and present in the majority of DLB cases. This hypothesis
was clearly demonstrated (1). However,...
I have read with great interest the article by Edison et al about the
amyloid load in patients with Parkinson´s disease (PD), PD with dementia
(PDD) and dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) (1). Authors hypothesised that
amyloid pathology would be uncommon in PD without dementia, an occasional
feature of PDD and present in the majority of DLB cases. This hypothesis
was clearly demonstrated (1). However, there are some points which are at
variance with previous imaging and pathological data. I am going to focus
in two of them which can be considered as paradigmatic of similar studies.
The first study used [11C]PIB scan and showed that 22% of cognitively
normal people had amyloid deposition in their brains (2) (This percentage
was of 15% in another study including 20 subjects after adjusting for age
(3)). These subjects were comparable in terms of age and MMSE scores, to
the ones included in the study by Edison et al. However, [11C]PIB scans
were normal in all the 41 control individuals and 10 PD patients without
dementia studied. The second one refers to the pathological analysis of
the brains of patients of the Sidney cohort (4). In this study, between 50
and 80% of the patients with a clinical diagnosis of PDD showed amyloid
plaques in their brains. The shorter the duration of PD, the higher the
percentage of plaques. Amyloid plaques were also found in 83% of patients
dead with a clinical diagnosis of DLB (4).
The purpose of this letter is not to discuss the psysiopathological
basis of dementia associated with PD, the position of DLB in the clinical
spectrum of Lewy body disorders or the value of [11C]PIB scanning for the
preclinical detection of AD, but to point out the fact that these results
do not fit with previous imaging data obtained in a similar way, as well
as with pathological studies. These discordant aspects are not discussed
by Edison et al yet they have been used to raise some conclusions quite
relevant to the concepts of AD and PD, such as the possibility of
preclinical diagnosis and the contribution of aging and AD-like pathology
to the development of dementia in PD. Moreover, one of the authors took
part in one of the prior studies with [11C]PIB imaging (2). How to
reconcile these data? Is this a diagnostic problem or a technical issue?
Is [11C]PIB imaging useful to diagnose amyloid deposition in cognitively
normal subjects? Is [11C]PIB deposition the reflection of the presence of
amyloid plaques? Was the diagnosis of DLB or PDD incorrect? Are the
conclusions of the Sidney study valid or more than 50% of the patients
included in this cohort actually had DLB instead of PD or PDD? It is
necessary to clarify the relationships between symptomatology, imaging and
pathology in degenerative diseases to avoid misinterpretations.
References
1. Edison P, Rowe CC, Rinne JO, et al. Amyloid load in Parkinson’s disease dementia and Lewy Body dementia
measured with [11C]PIB PET.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008; doi:10.1136/jnnp.2007.127878
2. Rowe CC, Ng S, Ackermann U et al. Imaging beta-amyloid burden in aging and dementia.
Neurology 2007;68:1718-1725
3. Mintun MA, Larossa GN, Sheline YI et al. [11C]PIB in a nondemented population: potential antecedent marker of
Alzheimer disease.
Neurology 2006;67:446-452
4. Halliday G, Hely M, Reid W, Morris J. The progression of pathology in longitudinally followed patients with
Parkinson’s disease.
Acta Neuropathol 2008; 115:409–415
I read the article of Yoo et al [1] in the Journal with interest and
would like to offer another explanation as to the cause of delayed and
weaker response of the right ABP to transcranial magnetic stimulation of
the right hemisphere, i.e. interhemispheric diaschisis.
Firstly, there is substantial number of case reports documenting
ipsilateral paralysis in unilateral cranial lesions (e.g....
I read the article of Yoo et al [1] in the Journal with interest and
would like to offer another explanation as to the cause of delayed and
weaker response of the right ABP to transcranial magnetic stimulation of
the right hemisphere, i.e. interhemispheric diaschisis.
Firstly, there is substantial number of case reports documenting
ipsilateral paralysis in unilateral cranial lesions (e.g. subdural
hematomas) in the absence of any cerebral herniation [2, 3]. Second, the
cases described by Kernohan and Woltman were all involving the left
hemisphere [4], a finding that requires a physiological explanation not
afforded by the notch created by horizontal shifting of the midbrain in
such cases [1]. Third, laterality related (left-sided) weakness has been
described with callosotomies performed without the use of a retractor
causing injury to the right hemisphere (as an explanation of such events)
[5]. Lastly, earlier onset of activity in the hemisphere containing the
command center (left in vast majority of people) has been documented
repeatedly; most recently by Wisneski et al [6], indicating transcallosal
transfer of motor commands arising from the major hemisphere for
movements occurring in the nondominant side of the body; i.e. commands
that are implemented by the minor hemisphere [7].
In short, these observations suggest that the hemiparesis described
by Yoo et al was based on diaschitic injury (deafferentation) of the minor
hemisphere (in this case the left) in a patient wired as a left hander
(but ostensibly right handed) and that the Kernohan notch they described
was an irrelevant artifact. As described elsewhere, such cases are the
neural substrates of crossed aphasias and crossed nonaphasias [2].
REFERENCES
1. Yoo WK, Kim DS, Kwon YH, Jang SH. Kernohan's notch phenomenon
demonstrated by diffusion tensor imaging and transcranial magnetic
stimulation.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2008; 79:1295-1297.
2. Derakhshan I. Callosum and movement control: case reports.
Neurol
Res. 2003; 25:538-542.
3. Kersey RD. Acute Subdural Hematoma after a Reported Mild
Concussion: A Case Report.
J Athl Train. 1998; 33:264-268.
4. Peyser E, Doron Y. Ipsilateral hemiplegia in supratentorial space
occupying lesions.
Int Surg. 1966; 45:689-695.
5. Phillips J, Sakas DE. Anterior callosotomy for intractable
epilepsy: outcome in a series of twenty patients.
Br J Neurosurg. 1996;
10:351-356.
6. Wisneski KJ, Anderson N, Schalk G, Smyth M, Moran D, Leuthardt EC. Unique Cortical Physiology Associated With Ipsilateral Hand Movements and
Neuroprosthetic Implications.
Stroke. October 2008.
7. Derakhshan I. Laterality of motor control revisited:
directionality of callosal traffic and its rehabilitative implications.
Top Stroke Rehabil. 2005; 12:76-82.
The authors report that "those taking antidepressants had a higher
median (interquartile range) body mass ratio (25.0 (21.4-27.6) versus 22.0
(20.0-25.2) (p=0.03) and a shorter median (interquartile range) time on
the treadmill (8.6 (6-11) versus 11.0 (8-12) minutes) (p=0.02). They also
had a significantly lower mean (SD) peak VO2 (27.9 (9.0) versus 32.8 (7.0)
ml/kg/min) (p=0.02)." In the discussion...
The authors report that "those taking antidepressants had a higher
median (interquartile range) body mass ratio (25.0 (21.4-27.6) versus 22.0
(20.0-25.2) (p=0.03) and a shorter median (interquartile range) time on
the treadmill (8.6 (6-11) versus 11.0 (8-12) minutes) (p=0.02). They also
had a significantly lower mean (SD) peak VO2 (27.9 (9.0) versus 32.8 (7.0)
ml/kg/min) (p=0.02)." In the discussion section, the authors then say,
"body mass index has been previously noted in subjects reporting fatigue.
This particular association, when combined with both the known and this
study's association between antidepressants and body mass index, suggests
that weight reduction might be a useful treatment strategy in some
patients with CFS."
Given in particular, the mention of weight reduction, I find it
strange that at this stage the authors don't comment that their results
might influence prescribing patterns with regards to some CFS patients
i.e. the corollary of the sentence above would be that avoiding
antidepressants "might be a useful treatment strategy in some patients
with CFS". So I'm doing it now.
In my experience, antidepressants are being prescribed for all sorts
of symptoms in CFS patients: pain, sleep, IBS-type symptoms, mental
fatigue/cognitive problems, low blood pressure/orthostatic issues,
depression to name a few. Following the reasoning above, it would suggest
that other methods may be preferable.
Since this study was published, a study by Welsh researchers[1] had
some intereresting things to say on the use of antidepressants in CFS
patients.
In a study of 23 patients and 42 healthy controls, they found that:
"The serum [free Trp]/[CAA] ratio was 43% higher in CFS patients, due to a
48% higher [free Trp]. [Total Trp] was also significantly higher (by 19%)
in CFS patients, and, although the [total Trp]/[CAA] ratio did not differ
significantly between the control and patient groups, the difference
became significant when the results were co-varied with age and gender.
[CAA] was not significantly different between groups, but was
significantly lower in
females, compared to males, of the CFS patient group."
The authors go on to say:
"We have provisionally identified two subgroups of CFS patients, one with
normal serotonin and the other with a high serotonin status."
The discussion section of the paper explains the relevance of some of
the findings. It also re-iterates my point that antidepressants are often
used to try to treat various symptoms associated with CFS:
"If the present results are confirmed, our proposals should have important
implications for the pharmacotherapy of the CFS. Serotonin-modifying and
other antidepressants are prescribed to CFS patients to treat depression
(if present), pain and/or fatigue. However, CFS patients with serotonin
excess should not be exposed to, and are unlikely to benefit from,
serotonin-modifying antidepressants, particularly selective serotonin-re-
uptake inhibitors (SSRIs), to avoid a potential excessive serotonin
hyperactivity. Similarly, in view of the well known Trp-SSRI interaction,
the latter should not be prescribed to CFS patients with high Trp levels
or increased availability to the brain. By contrast, as suggested by a
meta-analysis of previous pharmacological trials (O'Malley et al., 1999)
and a survey of psychiatrists (Hickie et al., 1999), tricyclic
antidepressants have a greater likelihood of efficacy than SSRIs, are
preferred by patients with chronic pain, and have superior antinociceptive
properties in fibromyalgia (McQuay et al., 1996). Also, our clinical
impression suggests that tricyclic antidepressants improve sleep
disturbances in, and are better tolerated than SSRIs by, CFS patients."
An alternative explanation could be read into the association between
exercise tolerance and body mass index: the more severely affected
patients are with CFS, because of some (as yet unknown) disease process
associated with some or all cases of CFS, the less able they are to
exercise and so the harder it is for them to keep their weight to optimum
levels. (Or alternatively the disease process increases their weight by
some other means.)
Similarly muscle weakness (which also featured in regression models
of exercise tolerance) could be a marker of the severity of the disease
process in some or all cases of CFS - I say "some or all cases" because,
as Lane points out in the accompanying editorial[2], "it is increasingly
recognised that chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS) is heterogeneous... Failure
to recognise this heterogeneity prejudices attempts to understand CFS in
cross sectional studies." He pointed out that some research that had been
published at that time suggested "some patients with CFS have a form of
metabolic myopathy". He subsequently published a paper[3] which found
that "there is an association between abnormal lactate response to
exercise, reflecting impaired muscle energy metabolism, and the presence
of enterovirus sequences in muscle in a proportion of CFS patients." This
was found by combining a subanaerobic threshold exercise test (SATET) and
a RT-NPCR test for viruses: "Muscle biopsy samples from 20.8% of the CFS
patients were positive for enterovirus sequences by RT-NPCR, while all the
29 control samples were negative; 58.3% of the CFS patients had a SATET+
response. Nine of the 10 enterovirus positive cases were among the 28
SATET+ patients (32.1%), compared with only one (5%) of the 20 SATET-
patients. PCR products were most closely related to coxsackie B virus."
An empirically derived definition for "ME/CFS"[4] has included
"generalized muscle weakness". "Muscle Weakness" has also been used in
other definitions of ME[5]. (CFS and ME are terms that are sometimes used
interchangeably).
It would have been interesting if information on the results of those
who had done graded exercise therapy had been included in this study, both
at the end of the 12 week intervention and at follow-up. There are a lot
of references to the trial but the paper itself[6] only gives bits and
pieces of the data of measures included in the current study.
It is interesting to note that the trial[6] did not prove graded
exercise therapy was a panacea for CFS, which has been demonstrated
numerically in a recent meta-analysis[7], which analysed the data (the
authors of the meta-analysis reported that they contacted authors of the
studies for missing data so they may have had access to data we are not
given).
For the studies under review, the Cohen's d value (an effect measure)
was calculated using the following method:
"Separate mean effect sizes were calculated for each category of outcome
variable (e.g., fatigue self- rating) and for each type of outcome
variable (mental, physical, and mixed mental and physical). Studies
generally included multiple outcome measures. For all analyses except
those that compared different categories or types of outcome variables, we
used the mean effect size of all the relevant outcome variables of the
study."
Malouff et al[7] calculated the d value for the study[6] as 0.46 (95%
CI: 0.03-0.95). For anyone unfamiliar with Cohen's d values, they are not
bounded by 1; also, the higher the score, the bigger the "effect size"
i.e. the more "effective" a treatment was found to be. Cohen's d values
are considered to be a small effect size at 0.2, a moderate effect size at
0.5, and a large effect size at 0.8[7].
That Graded Exercise Therapy is not a panacea for CFS is not
surprising given the aforementioned association Lane found[3] between
enteroviruses and abnormalities on exercise testing in some CFS patients.
REFERENCES
[1] Badawy AA, Morgan CJ, Llewelyn MB, Albuquerque SR, Farmer A. Heterogeneity of serum tryptophan concentration and availability to the
brain in patients with the chronic fatigue syndrome.
J Psychopharmacol.
2005 Dec;19(4):385-91.
[2] Lane R. Chronic fatigue syndrome: is it physical?
J Neurol
Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2000 Sep;69(3):289.
[3] Lane RJ, Soteriou BA, Zhang H, Archard LC Enterovirus related
metabolic myopathy: a postviral fatigue syndrome.
J Neurol Neurosurg
Psychiatry. 2003 Oct;74(10):1382-6.
[4] Osoba T, Pheby D, Gray S, Nacul L. The Development of an
Epidemiological Definition for Myalgic Encephalomyelitis/Chronic Fatigue
Syndrome.
Journal
of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome, Vol. 14(4), 2007
[5] Goudsmit EM, Stouten B, Howes S. Fatigue in Myalgic
Encephalomyelitis.
Bulletin of the IACFS/ME - Volume 16, Issue 3.
http://www.iacfsme.org/BULLETINFALL2008/Fall08GoudsmitFatigueinMyalgicEnceph/tabid/292/Default.aspx
[6] Fulcher KY, White PD Randomised controlled trial of graded
exercise in patients with the chronic fatigue syndrome.
BMJ 1997, 314:1647
-1652.
[7] Malouff, J. M., et al. Efficacy of cognitive behavioral therapy
for chronic fatigue syndrome: A meta-analysis.
Clinical Psychology Review
(2007), doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.004
[8] Cohen J Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences.
Edited by: 2. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1988.
We have read with interest the article by Soulas and colleagues
entitled "Attempted and completed suicides after subthalamic nucleus
stimulation for Parkinson's disease". (1)The authors reported a committed
suicide rate of at least 1% and a 2% attempted suicide rate in a
retrospective cohort of Parkinson's disease (PD) patients who underwent
bilateral subthalamic deep brain stimulation (STN DBS). T...
We have read with interest the article by Soulas and colleagues
entitled "Attempted and completed suicides after subthalamic nucleus
stimulation for Parkinson's disease". (1)The authors reported a committed
suicide rate of at least 1% and a 2% attempted suicide rate in a
retrospective cohort of Parkinson's disease (PD) patients who underwent
bilateral subthalamic deep brain stimulation (STN DBS). They did not find
any differences between suicidal patients and those without suicide
attempts. We note that among their 6 suicidal patients, 2 had PD onset
younger than 50 years (36 and 44 years) and that both the patients who
successfully committed suicide had a family history of PD. In view of this
and since the probability of a genetic form of PD is higher among young
onset PD patients,(2 3) it would be interesting to know the genetic
findings of the suicidal patients in their cohort.
Autosomal recessive PD is not rare among patients who undergo STN
DBS. Moro and colleagues screened 80 patients with disease onset at age 45
years or younger and bilateral STN DBS for mutations in the parkin
(PARK2), PINK1 (PARK6) and LRRK2 (PARK8) genes. They identified 12
mutation carriers (11 Parkin [6 homozygous, 5 heterozygous] and 1
homozygous PINK1).(4)
Previous literature has shown that heterozygous parkin as well as
PINK1 mutation carriers often develop not only motor but also
neuropsychiatric symptoms.(5-8) Susceptibility to psychiatric illness was
also reported in asymptomatic parkin mutation carriers. Khan and
colleagues found 17 % of psychiatric disturbances in relatives of 16
unrelated parkin disease patients.(9) In this context, it is of interest
that a locus for schizophrenia has been mapped to chromosome 6q25 adjacent
to PARK2.(10) Steinlechner and colleagues found predominantly affective
and schizophrenia spectrum disorders in 61% of PINK1 mutation carriers and
in 20% of mutation-negative cases in a large family with monogenic PD.(6)
They suggested that affective and psychotic symptoms may be part of the
phenotypic spectrum or even the sole manifestation of PINK1 mutations.
This hypothesis is further supported by Funayama and colleagues'
observations that digenic parkin - PINK1 mutation carrier patients are
more likely to manifest psychiatric symptoms.(11)
With respect to autosomal dominant PD, the literature describing STN-DBS
in LRRK2 patients is sparse and psychiatric disturbances have not been
systematically reported.(12 13) We have treated 6 G2019S LRRK2 patients
with STN-DBS, of whom, 4 had no psychiatric complications, while 2
developed severe psychiatric / affective complications resulting in a poor
functional outcome despite marked motor improvement.
Given that 2 of the 6 suicidal patients in this cohort had a family
history of PD (both of those who committed suicide) and that there may be
an association between parkin and/or PINK1 or LRRK2 mutations and
psychiatric manifestations, we suggest determining the genetic status of
the suicidal patients in this cohort with respect to the above mentioned
mutations. There is growing awareness of non-motor (psychiatric and
cognitive) morbidity associated with STN DBS and its negative impact on
functional outcome often despite much reduced motor disability, yet there
is no consensus on the risk factors or exclusion criteria for STN DBS
relevant to neuropsychiatric complications. Potentially, patients with
certain genetic mutations (or mutation combinations) may be predisposed to
such complications and therefore require additional caution with respect
to pre-operative assessment and counselling and post-operative monitoring.
REFERENCES
1. Soulas T, Gurruchaga JM, Palfi S, Cesaro P, Nguyen JP, Fénelon G. Attempted and completed suicides after subthalamic nucleus stimulation for
Parkinson's disease.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008;79:952-4.
2. Lücking CB, Dürr A, Bonifati V, Vaughan J, De Michele G, Gasser T, Harhangi, Biswadjiet S., Meco, Giuseppe, Denefle, Patrice, Wood, Nicholas W., Agid, Yves, Brice, Alexis, The European Consortium on Genetic
Susceptibility in Parkinson's Disease and The French Parkinson's Disease
Genetics Study Group Association between Early-Onset Parkinson's Disease
and Mutations in the Parkin Gene.
N Engl J Med 2000;42:1560-1567.
3. Tanner CM, Ottman R, Goldman SM, Ellenberg J, Chan P, Mayeux R,
Langston JW. Parkinson Disease in Twins: An Etiologic Study.
JAMA
1999;281(4):341-346.
4. Moro E, Volkmann J, König IR, Winkler S, Hiller A, Hassin-Baer S,
Herzog J, Schnitzler A, Lohmann K, Pinsker MO, Voges J, Djarmatic A,
Seibler P, Lozano AM, Rogaeva E, Lang AE, Deuschl G, Klein C. Bilateral
subthalamic stimulation in Parkin and PINK1 parkinsonism.
Neurology
2008;70:1186-91.
5. Kubo S, Hattori N, Mizuno Y. Recessive Parkinson's disease.
Mov
Disord 2006;21:885-93.
6. Steinlechner S, Stahlberg J, Völkel B, Djarmati A, Hagenah J,
Hiller A, Hedrich K, König I, Klein C, Lencer R. Co-occurrence of
affective and schizophrenia spectrum disorders with PINK1 mutations.
J
Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2007;78:532-535.
7. Ephraty L, Porat O, Israeli D, Cohen OS, Tunkel O, Yael S, Hatano
Y, Hattori N, Hassin-Baer S. Neuropsychiatric and cognitive features in
autosomal-recessive early parkinsonism due to PINK1 mutations.
Mov Disord
2007; 22:566-9.
8. Inzelberg R, Hattori N, Mizuno Y. Dopaminergic dysfunction in
unrelated, asymptomatic carriers of a single parkin mutation.
Neurology
2005;65;1843.
9. Khan NL, Graham E, Critchley P, Schrag AE, Wood NW, Lees AJ,
Bhatia KP, Quinn N. Parkin disease: a phenotypic study of a large case
series.
Brain 2003;126 :1279-92.
10. Lindholm E, Ekholm B, Shaw S, Jalonen P, Johansson G, Pettersson
U, Sherrington R, Adolfsson R, Jazin E. A Schizophrenia-Susceptibility
Locus at 6q25, in One of the World's Largest Reported Pedigrees.
Am J Hum
Genet 2001;69:96–105.
11. Funayama M, Li Y, Tsoi TH, Lam CW, Ohi T, Yazawa S, Uyama E,
Djaldetti R, Melamed E, Yoshino H, Imamichi Y, Takashima H, Nishioka K,
Sato K, Tomiyama H, Kubo S, Mizuno Y, Hattori N. Familial Parkinsonism
with digenic parkin and PINK1 mutations.
Mov Disord 2008;23:1461-5.
12. Gómez-Esteban JC, Lezcano E, Zarranz JJ, González C, Bilbao G,
Lambarri I, Rodríguez O, Garibi J. Outcome of bilateral deep brain
subthalamic stimulation in patients carrying the R1441G mutation in the
LRRK2 dardarin gene.
Neurosurgery 2008;62:857-62.
13. Schüpbach M, Lohmann E, Anheim M, Lesage S, Czernecki V, Yaici S,
Worbe Y, Charles P, Welter ML, Pollak P, Dürr A, Agid Y, Brice A. Subthalamic nucleus stimulation is efficacious in patients with
Parkinsonism and LRRK2 mutations.
Mov Disord 2007; 22:119-122.
Dear Editor
I am pleased to read the comments by Dr. Derakhshan of 27
August, 2008. I would like to reply some questions in following
paragraphs.
First, Dr. Derakhshan wrote that it was hard to understand that the
sensory defects of our patient were detected on the right side of the body
by the callosal infarction, and this might have been a typographical or
clerical error. If you mentioned the somaesthetic...
Dear Editor
I am pleased to read the comments by Dr. Derakhshan of 27
August, 2008. I would like to reply some questions in following
paragraphs.
First, Dr. Derakhshan wrote that it was hard to understand that the
sensory defects of our patient were detected on the right side of the body
by the callosal infarction, and this might have been a typographical or
clerical error. If you mentioned the somaesthetic transfer defect, I�fm
afraid that you might misunderstand the method of its evaluation. This
method requires no verbal answer. As shown in the paper, I touched a
point on the patient�fs hand with a pen while the patient�fs eyes were
closed, and then asked him to touch the corresponding place on the
ipsilateral and contralateral hand using his thumb. When I touched the
patient�fs left hand, the patient correctly identified 16/16 on the left
hand, namely on the ipsilateral hand to the stimulated hand, and 6/16 on
the right hand, namely on the contralateral hand to the stimulated hand.
When the patient replied by his left hand to the stimulus on his left
hand, the sensory input to right hemisphere was connected to the motor
area in the same, right hemisphere. But, when the patient replied by his
right hand to the stimulus on his left hand, the information of the touch
by a pen on the patient�fs left hand must be transferred from right to
left hemisphere, in order to touch corresponding place using his thumb of
his right hand. The results were significantly lowered in the latter case
(left stim./left reply 16/16, left stim./right reply 6/16, p=0.0002).
This means the sensory input to right hemisphere could not be transferred
to left hemisphere, due to the callosal infarction. So, we may say that
the patient revealed the somaesthetic transfer defect.
Second, Dr. Derakhshan stated that, in violin playing, the right hand
also played a critical role to produce the sounds. I completely agree
this opinion, but, in our patient, I suppose that the impairment of violin
playing was due to the impairment of the movements of his left fingers.
As written in the paper, the patient complained that, after his
cerebrovascular event, he could not play violin as well as before, because
his left fingers could not move with their previous accuracy. For
example, when he pressed the string by his index finger of left hand, the
middle finger also moved involuntarily and touched the string, so the
sound became very vague. Then, he complained that independent movements
of his left fingers were damaged after the cerebrovascular event. At the
same time, he told that he felt no abnormal change about the bowing by
right hand, compared to before the infarction. We suppose that the
impairment of the movement of left fingers was caused by the callosal
apraxia. It is generally considered that left hemisphere is also dominant
about voluntary movements, so the callosal apraxia appears only in the
left hand, because the disconnection between the praxic center of left
hemisphere and the motor area of right hemisphere. Though the subtle
disorder of bowing movement might also participate, I suppose that it is
reasonably conclude that, as the patient stated by himself, the impairment
of his violin playing was mainly caused by the disturbance of the movement
of his left fingers due to the callosal apraxia.
Third, Dr. Derakhshan wrote that the patient�fs left hemianopia for
musical symbols which was displayed in tachistoscopic examinations was due
to the difficulty in moving his eyes to the left in the time allowed. I
wonder if Dr. Derakhshan might misunderstand the meaning of tachistoscopic
examination. As shown in our paper, in tachistoscope, the visual stimuli
were presented for 70ms in each visual hemifield, beside a yellow spot 3
degrees in visual angle. As you know, the tachistoscopic examination is
carried out in order to measure the responses of each cerebral hemisphere
independently. The duration of 70ms is set in order NOT to allow the time
to move eyes. So, if the patient has the enough time to move his eyes as
Dr. Derakhshan said, the visual stimuli are input to both hemispheres.
Therefore, we may say that our tachistoscopic examination was
methodologically adequate and its results were reliable.
Dr. Derakhshan also wrote that the splenium of corpus callosum seemed
intact. As far as we identified the series of MRI images of the patient,
we can state that the anterior half of the splenium was damaged.
I am afraid that I could not understand your useful questions
correctly, because of my poor English. If so, I'll be happy if you point
out my mistakes, again.
We read with interest the paper by Leung et al. 1 in which they
investigated the agreement between the initial diagnosis/ labels (seizures
versus non-specific initial labels like dizziness, syncope and collapse)
made at the initial accident and emergency department evaluation and the
subsequent final diagnosis (seizure versus non-epileptic event) after
inpatient neurological evaluation. As the autho...
We read with interest the paper by Leung et al. 1 in which they
investigated the agreement between the initial diagnosis/ labels (seizures
versus non-specific initial labels like dizziness, syncope and collapse)
made at the initial accident and emergency department evaluation and the
subsequent final diagnosis (seizure versus non-epileptic event) after
inpatient neurological evaluation. As the authors rightly comment
obtaining a thorough history both from the patient and the eye-witness,
judicious use of EEG (routine and in some cases prolonged inpatient or
outpatient monitoring) and a specialist consultation aid in establishing
an accurate diagnosis in most patients. I want to add the utility of the
now ubiquitous cell phone to this armament of technology at our disposal
for diagnosing seizures. Most of the cell phones in the market today have
video taking capability and in my practice I have more than once been
surprised when on asking the eye-witness to describe the event in
question, I actually got to see a small video clip of the same. Clinical
semiology greatly aids in differentiating between epileptic and non-
epileptic events especially to the trained eye. Neurologists should
embrace this cheap effective tool and harness its power by encouraging
patients and eye-witness to record these paroxysmal events.
References
1. Leung H, Man CY, Hui ACF, Wong KS, Kwan P. Agreement between
initial and final diagnosis of first seizures, epilepsy and non-epileptic
events: a prospective study.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008; 79:1144-
1147.
Zephir et al. (1) report on five patients with a syndrome of combined
central and peripheral demyelination, characterized by (i) immunological
aetiology, proved by CSF findings, nerve biopsy examination, and response
to immune treatments; and (ii) recurrent disease course, with central
nervous system (CNS) involvement apparently preceding the onset of
peripheral nervous system (PNS) damage by severa...
Zephir et al. (1) report on five patients with a syndrome of combined
central and peripheral demyelination, characterized by (i) immunological
aetiology, proved by CSF findings, nerve biopsy examination, and response
to immune treatments; and (ii) recurrent disease course, with central
nervous system (CNS) involvement apparently preceding the onset of
peripheral nervous system (PNS) damage by several years (2 to 12).
Screenings for systemic autoimmunity and infections were both negative,
and only in 2/5 patients a chronic inflammatory demyelinating
polyradiculoneuropathy (CIDP) was hypothesized, possibly related to
interferon (IFN) treatment. The Authors conclude for an immune reaction
directed against an antigen common to both CNS and PNS.
As the Authors state, and according to our own experience, several
hallmarks of acute CNS demyelination distinguish this syndrome from
multiple sclerosis (MS), including (i) clinical features, such as
bilateral optic neuropathy, transverse myelitis with or without roots
enhancement, advanced age of onset; (ii) CSF features, such as absence of
oligoclonal bands and marked increase in protein levels; and (iii) MRI
features, such as grey matter involvement, mass effect, and multiple
enhancing lesions.
On the other hand, peripheral demyelination was not typical for CIDP since
the EMG failed to reveal temporal dispersion/conduction blocks, whereas
showing marked damage of sensory fibres. We note that the CIDP cases
associated with CNS involvement, that are cited by Zephir et al. in
support of their hypothesis, differ from those of their own series in two
important aspects, that is, the predominance of PNS vs. CNS symptoms, and
the simultaneous involvement of both CNS and PNS.
The latter is a crucial point: considering a relapsing immune-mediated
disease with common CNS/PNS target antigens, then a relapse episode would
indeed be expected to produce simultaneous PNS/CNS damage. Such a
combination is known to occur with certain infectious agents, e.g.
Mycoplasma pneumoniae, or in post-infectious disorders.
However, this was
not the case for the patients reported by Zephir et al, who failed to show
signs of infections, with the exception of a possible enteritis in patient
3. In such cases, characterized by “temporal dissemination” of lesions
between CNS and PNS, a multifocal, multisystemic autoimmune disorder is a
more likely diagnosis. We wonder whether screening for autoimmune
disorders was repeated during the relapses, since this search can be
entirely negative during the first episode.
Although the Authors exclude the possibility of acute disseminated
encephalomyelitis (ADEM) based on the absence of a clinically evident
antecedent infection, and in view of the temporal succession of symptoms
in their patients, we question this conclusion, since recently reported
ADEM variants (2) do include forms with PNS involvement as well as
relapsing forms.
For instance, in 60 patients with ADEM prospectively collected through 5
years, we made two observations that are relevant for the present
discussion.
First, PNS involvement, that was systematically searched for in all
patients, irrespective of their clinical features, did occur in 26/60
patients (43%), with a demyelinating pattern in 17/26 (65%); however, it
was symptomatic in only 10/26 patients (38%), due to the predominance of
CNS involvement that masked the PNS symptoms. Likewise, in the series
reported by Zephir et al. subclinical PNS involvement could well be
present right since disease onset, but became clinically manifest later
on, once the CNS symptoms began to subside. The only exception may be
patient 2, who had a negative EMG examination at the onset of CNS
demyelination.
Second, PNS involvement was associated with higher risk of relapse in our
patients (2, 3). This can also explain the high relapse rate and long-term
disability of the patients reported by Zephir et al.
These observations support the notion that, although ADEM is considered a
monophasic disease of the CNS, either relapses or PNS damage may occur,
and often co-occur, in this condition.
In conclusion, we propose that screening for PNS involvement should be
given to all patients with “atypical MS”, irrespective of the presence of
peripheral symptoms. The elevated risk of relapse and long-term disability
should be considered in deciding a prophylactic treatment, since IFN could
be contraindicated or ineffective (4). In the acute phase, agents active
on both CNS and PNS demyelination, such as IVIg, may be first choice in
these cases.
REFERENCES
1. Zéphir H, Stojkovic T, Latour P, Lacour A, de Seze J, Outteryck O,
Maurage CA,
Monpeurt C, Chatelet P, Ovelacq E, Vermersch P. Relapsing demyelinating
disease affecting both the central and peripheral nervous systems.
J
Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2008 Sep;79(9):1032-9. Epub 2008 Jan 21.
2. Marchioni E, Ravaglia S, Piccolo G, Furione M, Zardini E,
Franciotta D, Alfonsi
E, Minoli L, Romani A, Todeschini A, Uggetti C, Tavazzi E, Ceroni M. Postinfectious inflammatory disorders: subgroups based on prospective
follow-up.
Neurology. 2005 Oct 11;65(7):1057-65.
3. Ravaglia S, Piccolo G, Ceroni M, Franciotta D, Pichiecchio A,
Bastianello S,
Tavazzi E, Minoli L, Marchioni E. Severe steroid-resistant post-infectious
encephalomyelitis: general features and effects of IVIg.
J Neurol. 2007
Nov;254(11):1518-23. Epub 2007 Nov 14.
4. Matsuse D, Ochi H, Tashiro K, Nomura T, Murai H, Taniwaki T, Kira
J. Exacerbation of chronic inflammatory demyelinating polyradiculoneuropathy
during
interferonbeta-1b therapy in a patient with childhood-onset multiple
sclerosis.
Intern Med. 2005 Jan;44(1):68-72.
Dear Editor,
We read with great interest the article by Fukuda et al. “Facial nerve motor-evoked potential monitoring during skull base surgery predicts facial nerve outcome.” (J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008; 79:1066-1070).
In this rather small group of 26 patients with skull base tumors, the authors have tried to establish a role for facial nerve motor evoked potentials (FNMEPs) to predicting the neuro...
Dear Editor,
Multiple sclerosis is believed to be an autoimmune pathology, yet the mechanisms triggering the disease remain elusive. Therefore, I read with great interest the paper by Zamboni and his team who investigated the venous hemodynamics in patients with multiple sclerosis. His findings that this disease might be attributable to venous refluxes shed new light on the facts that have been known for decades...
Dear Editor,
We are writing in response to the meta-analysis by Doubal, Hokke and Wardlaw “Retinal Microvascular Abnormalities and Stroke – A Systematic Review” published online on 17 October 2008. While the paper is well written, the topic interesting and potentially important, we have several concerns regarding the methodological rigor of the meta-analysis.
The fundamental issue is inappropriate poo...
Dear Editor,
I have read with great interest the article by Edison et al about the amyloid load in patients with Parkinson´s disease (PD), PD with dementia (PDD) and dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB) (1). Authors hypothesised that amyloid pathology would be uncommon in PD without dementia, an occasional feature of PDD and present in the majority of DLB cases. This hypothesis was clearly demonstrated (1). However,...
Dear Editor,
I read the article of Yoo et al [1] in the Journal with interest and would like to offer another explanation as to the cause of delayed and weaker response of the right ABP to transcranial magnetic stimulation of the right hemisphere, i.e. interhemispheric diaschisis.
Firstly, there is substantial number of case reports documenting ipsilateral paralysis in unilateral cranial lesions (e.g....
Dear Editor,
The authors report that "those taking antidepressants had a higher median (interquartile range) body mass ratio (25.0 (21.4-27.6) versus 22.0 (20.0-25.2) (p=0.03) and a shorter median (interquartile range) time on the treadmill (8.6 (6-11) versus 11.0 (8-12) minutes) (p=0.02). They also had a significantly lower mean (SD) peak VO2 (27.9 (9.0) versus 32.8 (7.0) ml/kg/min) (p=0.02)." In the discussion...
Dear Editor,
We have read with interest the article by Soulas and colleagues entitled "Attempted and completed suicides after subthalamic nucleus stimulation for Parkinson's disease". (1)The authors reported a committed suicide rate of at least 1% and a 2% attempted suicide rate in a retrospective cohort of Parkinson's disease (PD) patients who underwent bilateral subthalamic deep brain stimulation (STN DBS). T...
Dear Editor I am pleased to read the comments by Dr. Derakhshan of 27 August, 2008. I would like to reply some questions in following paragraphs.
First, Dr. Derakhshan wrote that it was hard to understand that the sensory defects of our patient were detected on the right side of the body by the callosal infarction, and this might have been a typographical or clerical error. If you mentioned the somaesthetic...
Dear Editor,
We read with interest the paper by Leung et al. 1 in which they investigated the agreement between the initial diagnosis/ labels (seizures versus non-specific initial labels like dizziness, syncope and collapse) made at the initial accident and emergency department evaluation and the subsequent final diagnosis (seizure versus non-epileptic event) after inpatient neurological evaluation. As the autho...
Dear Editor,
Zephir et al. (1) report on five patients with a syndrome of combined central and peripheral demyelination, characterized by (i) immunological aetiology, proved by CSF findings, nerve biopsy examination, and response to immune treatments; and (ii) recurrent disease course, with central nervous system (CNS) involvement apparently preceding the onset of peripheral nervous system (PNS) damage by severa...
Pages