Elsevier

Brain and Language

Volume 46, Issue 4, May 1994, Pages 536-564
Brain and Language

Regular Article
Quantitative and Qualitative Evaluation of Patterns of Cerebral Language Dominance: An Amobarbital Study

https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.1994.1030Get rights and content

Abstract

As a part of presurgical evaluation, 173 patients received bilateral intracarotid amobarbital tests for determination of cerebral language dominance. Language testing during intracarotid amobarbital procedures (IAP) consisted of the following tasks: automatic speech, sentence comprehension, body commands, naming, repetition, reading, and spontaneous speech. Patterns of cerebral language dominance were evaluated and discussed on five levels of analysis: (1) quantification of language dominance on the basis of a lateralization index derived from the total language scores in each IAP; (2) determination of five dominance subpatterns (left or right dominant, strongly bilateral, and incomplete left or right dominant) according to quantification performed on level (I) and clinical judgment; (3) qualitative differentiation of three kinds of bilaterality (positive, negative, and general) according to total language performance in left and right IAP; (4) analysis of grouped linguistic subfunctions extracted from performance in specific IAP subtests; (5) extraordinary individual case histories. The distribution of lateralization indices revealed only partially continuous degrees of lateralization, especially between the left-dominant and bilateral subgroups. As for the clinically oriented classification, incomplete left dominance is frequent (16.2%), while incomplete right dominance does not occur at all. Atypical dominance patterns are mostly correlated to bilateral and/or extratemporal foci. Concerning grouped subfunctions, a rotated factor matrix statistic yields an analysis of clusters of IAP subtests, where functions involving expressive language capacities are separated from those that are purely receptive. Further analyses of bilaterality subpatterns suggest that there are mainly four bilaterality phenomena, namely interhemispheric dissociation, double representation, unilateal representation of subfunctions, and partial representation of subfunctions in either hemisphere. Application of these differentiations to individual cases yields additional evidence that can be used in patient selection for operation in order to avoid postoperative neuropsychological deficits, especially in candidates for extratemporal surgery. In conclusion, a multilevel analysis of LAP language data is recommended since it permits a detailed account of varieties of language dominance patterns and contributes to more adequate presurgical decision-making in planned operations in cognitively relevant brain areas.

References (0)

Cited by (91)

  • Brain structural changes in spasmodic dysphonia: A multimodal magnetic resonance imaging study

    2016, Parkinsonism and Related Disorders
    Citation Excerpt :

    On the other hand, the right hemisphere usually supports other aspects of language production, such as prosody and pragmatic processing [29]. Several studies, however, have challenged this dichotomy suggesting that, even in right-handers, language representation can be right-sided or bilaterally distributed [30–33]. A right lateralization of FA alterations has been also previously reported in SD [8], as well as in a fMRI study of patients with adductor type SD [4].

  • Intracarotid Sodium Amytal Test and Language

    2015, International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences: Second Edition
  • Bilateral representation of language: A critical review and analysis of some unusual cases

    2014, Journal of Neurolinguistics
    Citation Excerpt :

    The frontal areas are homologous, but only the right insula is involved. This subject might have a bilateral positive (double representation) of Kurthen et al. (1994), where an incomplete loss of language may be expected with a left carotid injection and no impairment with a right carotid injection, provided there is redundancy of the Broca's area. For Loring et al. (1990) and Möddel et al. (2009) this case will also be a bilateral dependent type.

View all citing articles on Scopus
View full text