figure a

Introduction

Dementia is a major public health problem [1]. The burden of poor metabolic health is also rising worldwide and is a strong risk factor for dementia [1]. The presence of type 2 diabetes mellitus, in particular, doubles the risk of dementia in older age [2]. Findings from the majority of longitudinal studies suggest that type 2 diabetes is associated with a greater decline in executive function [3,4,5], processing speed [4, 6,7,8], verbal fluency [6] and memory [3, 4, 6], while the results of a few studies suggest no associations in these domains [3, 5, 9].

There is great interest in identifying pathways linking diabetes and cognitive decline. Type 2 diabetes is associated with lower total brain volume [10], more infarcts [11, 12] and greater white matter hyperintensity (WMH) volume [11, 12]. Cross-sectional analyses suggest that lower grey matter volume [11, 13] may substantially mediate the association between type 2 diabetes and cognitive function. However, in the Action to Control Cardiovascular Risk in Diabetes (ACCORD)–Memory in Diabetes (MIND) trial, the less intensive blood pressure and more intensive glycaemic control arms resulted in greater preservation of brain volume but not cognition [14, 15]. In the handful of longitudinal analyses on this topic, type 2 diabetes has been linked to an increase in ventricular volume [16,17,18]; a greater decline in total brain volume has been reported in some [19] but not all studies [17, 18, 20]. To our knowledge, there have been no longitudinal studies exploring whether brain atrophy mediates the association between type 2 diabetes and cognitive decline.

In a sample of community-dwelling older people without a history of dementia, we hypothesised that: (1) people with type 2 diabetes would have a greater rate of decline in cognition and total brain volume, and a greater increase in ventricular volume, compared with non-diabetic individuals; (2) these measures of brain atrophy would mediate associations between type 2 diabetes and cognitive decline.

Methods

Study population

Participants aged ≥55 years were recruited from the National Diabetes Service Scheme register in Southern Tasmania (postcodes 7000–7199) into the Cognition and Diabetes in Older Tasmanians (CDOT) longitudinal study between January 2008 and January 2010. The study was designed to examine the effect of type 2 diabetes on cognition and its underlying brain pathways. The National Diabetes Service Scheme register is run by Diabetes Australia and provides support to people with type 2 diabetes. Diagnosis is made by a physician using standard criteria (fasting plasma glucose ≥7.0 mmol/l, random plasma glucose ≥11.1 mmol/l or 2 h plasma glucose ≥11.1 mmol/l after an oral glucose tolerance test). Participants without type 2 diabetes were recruited from the longitudinal population-based Tasmanian Study of Cognition and Gait (TASCOG) and randomly selected using the Southern Tasmanian electoral roll from the same postcodes between December 2004 and 2010. In both studies participants were contacted by letter asking them to participate; they then attended an appointment at the University of Tasmania. Exclusion criteria were residence in a nursing home, insufficient English for cognitive testing, any contraindication to MRI scan, and a history of dementia or Parkinson’s disease (determined by a standardised questionnaire). Incident type 2 diabetes at follow-up was defined as diagnosis by a physician, HbA1c ≥ 48 mmol/mol (6.5%) or fasting plasma glucose ≥7.0 mmol/l.

Ethics approval was obtained from the Southern Tasmanian Health and Medical Human Research Ethics Committee and the Monash University Human Research Ethics Committee. Informed written consent was obtained from all participants.

Measurements

Both groups were followed up approximately 2 and 4 years after the baseline assessment. Measurement of all variables (including fasting blood tests) was identical in both cohorts.

Cognitive tests and derivation of cognitive scores

A comprehensive battery of neuropsychological tests was used to measure: (1) verbal fluency, using the Controlled Oral Word Association Test (with the letters F, A and S; category fluency, animals) [21]; (2) executive function interference, using the Victoria Stroop Test (and the colour minus word subtests) [22]; (3) working memory, using the digit span subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Third Edition [23]; (4) attention-processing speed, using the Victoria Stroop dot tests, symbol search and digit symbol-coding subtests of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Third Edition [23]; (5) visuospatial ability, using the Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test, copy task [21]; (6) verbal memory, using the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test–Revised generating scores for total immediate recall, delayed recall and recognition memory [21]; (7) visual memory, with a delayed reproduction after 20 min of the Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test [21]. For individual tests, scores were standardised at each visit by creating z scores using the mean and SD from the baseline visit. These domain scores were averaged to create a global cognitive score and also average scores for each of the seven listed cognitive domains. Domain scores with more than one cognitive test were restandardised to an SD of 1. These scores were used in the regression analysis to allow comparison of associations across cognitive domains, as has been done previously [3, 6, 9].

MRI brain (total brain and lateral ventricular volume)

Brain MRI prior to January 2011 was performed using a 1.5 Tesla scanner (LX Horizon; General Electric, Milwaukee, WI, USA) with the following sequences: high-resolution T1-weighted spoiled gradient echo (repetition time [TR] 35 ms, echo time [TE] 7 ms, flip angle 35°, field of view 240 × 240 mm, voxel size 1 mm3) comprising 120 contiguous slices; fluid-attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) (TR 8802 ms, TE 130 ms, inversion time 2200 ms, voxel size 0.50 × 0.50 × 3 mm). MRI after January 2011 was performed using a new 1.5 Tesla scanner (Syngo; Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) with the following sequences: high-resolution T1-weighted magnetisation-prepared gradient echo (MPRAGE) (TR 1910 ms, TE 3.14 ms, flip angle 15°, field of view 235 × 250 mm, voxel size 1 mm3) comprising 160 contiguous slices; FLAIR (TR 8500 ms, TE 92 ms, inversion time 2438 ms, voxel size 0.9 × 0.9 × 3.5 mm). Eleven participants were scanned on the new and old scanner and calibration factors were computed using linear regression, such that rates of change of volume between the previous time point and each calibration scan were matched. T1-weighted and FLAIR scans for each participant were aligned using the co-registration facility of SPM12 (www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). The FreeSurfer version 5.3 longitudinal pipeline [24] was used to estimate total brain volume, lateral ventricular volume and total intracranial volume with correction of intermediate stages of FreeSurfer segmentation for WMH masks created from FLAIR scans. WMH volume at baseline was computed using established segmentation procedures; MRI infarcts or cerebral microbleeds at baseline were determined by consensus between expert raters [25]. All image analyses were blinded to age, sex and cognitive outcome measures.

Covariates

Potential covariates included baseline age (centred to 55 years), sex, education (years) and self-reported vascular risk history of previous or current smoking (yes/no), myocardial infarct (yes/no), stroke (yes/no), hypertension (self-report, taking antihypertensive medication or having a systolic blood pressure [SBP] ≥140 mmHg or a diastolic blood pressure [DBP] ≥90 mmHg), hypercholesterolaemia (yes/no), BMI, depression (Geriatric Depression Scale) and ApoE4 (also known as APOE) genotype (whole blood DNA).

Statistics

Non-normal variables were transformed as required. Differences between diabetes status and baseline characteristics were examined using t tests and χ2 tests. For longitudinal analyses, mixed models (maximum likelihood estimation, unstructured covariance) were used to examine the associations between baseline diabetes status and change in MRI brain measures, global cognitive function and the individual cognitive domains. Fixed effects were terms for time since baseline and main effects were for diabetes status and an interaction between diabetes and time. Random effects for the intercept and slope were fitted for each individual, allowing participants to have different scores at baseline and rates of change in the dependent variable (MRI brain or cognitive measures). Model 1 was adjusted for baseline age, sex and education (and total intracranial volume for MRI measures). Model 2 included the covariates at baseline and their interaction with time in order to adjust for their effect on the relationship between type 2 diabetes and MRI brain/cognition over time. Covariates in model 2 were included if they changed the coefficient of the diabetes × time interaction by more than 10%. Three-way interactions were also explored between type 2 diabetes, time and the following covariates: age (greater than or less than 65 years of age), sex and ApoE4 status. For MRI outcome variables, additional three-way interactions were explored for baseline WMH volume, infarcts and microbleeds.

To examine whether MRI markers of atrophy mediated any associations between type 2 diabetes and decline in cognition, we entered the MRI variables and their interaction with time into models of type 2 diabetes and cognitive decline. As in our prior study, if the MRI measure attenuated the β coefficient for diabetes × time (by >30%), and the coefficient of the MRI measure remained unchanged from its unadjusted value without diabetes in the model, it was considered a potential mediator [11].

We performed three sensitivity analyses. First, we used multiple imputation to replace missing data during follow-up and repeated our analyses. Multiple imputation is likely to improve precision (reduce bias) when compared with more traditional approaches for dealing with missing data [26]. It is based on the assumption that the missing data are missing at random. Although missing at random is empirically unverifiable [27], individuals with cognitive impairment and other conditions at baseline were more likely to drop out of the study than were individuals without health conditions, suggesting that the probability of dropout depended on observed baseline measures but not the unobserved outcomes. Unrestricted model-based multiple imputation using Bayesian estimation [28] was used to impute the missing values because such a model is general enough so that model misspecification cannot occur [29]. The baseline measures (age, sex, education, type 2 diabetes status, ApoE4 genotype, vascular risk factors, cognitive measures and all MRI brain variables) were included, reducing the uncertainty caused by the missing values and therefore improving the precision of the estimation [30]. Multiple imputation involved taking five copies of the dataset [31] and imputing the missing values in each copy. The model was fitted separately to each of the five complete datasets, and the inference was carried out by combining the estimates and standard errors of each variable of interest across the five completed datasets using Rubin’s rules [32]. The resulting multiple imputation estimates are the average of the variable estimates across the five imputed datasets. We also performed ‘worst and best case’ scenarios by giving any missing outcome a value of 2 SDs below or above the sample mean. Analysis was performed using STATA version 15 (StataCorp, College Station, TX, USA).

Results

There were 705 participants in the study at phase 1 (diabetes, n = 348; no diabetes, n = 357). Phase 2 mean follow-up time was 2.6 years (SD 0.44, median 2.6, interquartile range 2.3–2.8) and phase 3 mean follow-up time was 4.6 years (SD 0.53, median 4.4, interquartile range 4.2–5.0). Five participants had missing cognitive data but did have MRI scans. There were 506 participants at phase 2 (two with missing cognitive data but with MRI scans) and 431 at phase 3 (one with missing cognitive data but with an MRI scan). MRI scans were performed in participants with no contraindications (claustrophobia or metal implants). The total number of MRI scans was as follows: phase 1, n = 602; phase 2, n = 385; phase 3, n = 295. Overall, those lost to follow-up were older (p < 0.001), had higher HbA1c (p = 0.01), were more likely to have had a myocardial infarct (p = 0.001) or stroke (p = 0.001), and had lower baseline total global cognition z score (p = 0.002), higher ventricular volume (p = 0.008) and lower brain volume (p = 0.001). There was no difference in dropout by type 2 diabetes status (p = 0.47), sex (p = 0.16), education (p = 0.31), hypertension (p = 0.94), high cholesterol (p = 0.33) or BMI (p = 0.85). Seven people developed incident type 2 diabetes during follow-up.

Participant characteristics by diabetes status are provided in Table 1. Those with type 2 diabetes were more likely to be younger (p < 0.001), on blood pressure- and lipid-lowering medication (p < 0.001), and have a history of high blood pressure and high cholesterol (p < 0.001). Those with type 2 diabetes also had a higher BMI (p < 0.001) and higher depression scores (p = 0.002). SBP (p = 0.001) and DBP (p < 0.001) were slightly higher in those without type 2 diabetes. The mean duration of diabetes was 9.5 years (SD 9.4); 71 participants were on insulin therapy.

Table 1 Participant characteristics (n = 705)

Type 2 diabetes and cognitive decline

At baseline, type 2 diabetes was associated with lower attention-processing speed (β −0.19; 95% CI −0.36, −0.02; p = 0.03), visuospatial ability (β −0.68; 95% CI −0.84, −0.53; p < 0.001) and visual memory (β −0.54; 95% CI −0.69, −0.38; p < 0.001), but not other cognitive variables (p > 0.05). Table 2 (model 1) shows the age-, sex- and education-adjusted associations between type 2 diabetes at baseline and cognitive decline over time. Type 2 diabetes was associated with a greater decline in the domains of verbal fluency (β −0.04; 95% CI −0.07, −0.02; p = 0.001) and verbal memory (β −0.05; 95% CI −0.08, −0.02; p = 0.002), and with a trend for working memory (β −0.02; 95% CI −0.05, −0.00; p = 0.05). Although visuospatial function and visual memory were lower in people with type 2 diabetes at baseline and subsequent assessments, they declined at a slower rate in people with type 2 diabetes (visuospatial function: β 0.14; 95% CI 0.11, 0.18; p < 0.001; visual memory: β 0.10; 95% CI 0.07, 0.13; p < 0.001). Type 2 diabetes was not associated with a greater rate of decline in global cognitive score (β −0.03; 95% CI −0.07, 0.01; p = 0.17), attention-processing speed (β −0.01; 95% CI −0.23, 0.01; p = 0.37) or executive function interference score (β −0.03; 95% CI −0.08, 0.02; p = 0.20).

Table 2 Difference in change over time between no diabetes and type 2 diabetes

In fully adjusted models (Table 2, model 2) the association between type 2 diabetes and greater decline in verbal fluency (β −0.03; 95% CI −0.06, −0.00) (Fig. 1) and verbal memory (β −0.06; 95% CI −0.09, −0.02) (Fig. 1), and between type 2 diabetes and slower decline in visuospatial ability (β 0.14; 95% CI 0.11, 0.18) and visual memory (β 0.11; 95% CI 0.08, 0.14) remained significant. The association with working memory was no longer significant (β −0.02; 95% CI −0.05, 0.01). Three-way interaction terms of diabetes and time did not suggest increased rates of decline in people of older age, ApoE4 carriers or either sex (p > 0.05). Results in all models did not change meaningfully when the seven participants with incident diabetes during follow-up were removed. Table 3 shows the associations after imputing missing data. There was little change in the strength of the majority of associations, except for verbal fluency, which weakened (β −0.02; 95% CI−0.06, 0.01). Table 4 shows the results of the worst-case scenario, where the association between type 2 diabetes and verbal fluency weakened slightly (β −0.01; 95% CI −0.05, 0.03). Electronic Supplementary Material (ESM) Table 1 shows the more unlikely best-case scenario, where the association between type 2 diabetes and verbal fluency was of similar strength but non-significant (β −0.03; 95% CI −0.08, 0.02).

Fig. 1
figure 1

(a) Longitudinal association between type 2 diabetes and verbal fluency (predictive margins). (b) Longitudinal association between type 2 diabetes and verbal memory (predictive margins). Unbroken line, no type 2 diabetes; broken line, type 2 diabetes

Table 3 Difference in change over time between no type 2 diabetes and type 2 diabetes with imputed data
Table 4 Difference in change over time between no type 2 diabetes and type 2 diabetes worst case scenario

Type 2 diabetes and brain atrophy

At baseline, type 2 diabetes was associated with lower total brain volume (β −14.273; 95% CI −21.197, −6.580; p < 0.001) and higher ventricular volume (β 2.672; 95% CI 0.152, 5.193; p = 0.04). Table 2 shows the associations between type 2 diabetes at baseline and MRI measures over time: model 1 is adjusted for baseline age, sex and education (as for the analysis of cognition), as well as total intracranial volume; model 2 is adjusted for the same covariates as for the analysis of cognition. Total brain volume decreased (β −8.481; 95% CI −9.863, −7.099) and lateral ventricular volume increased (β 1.142; 95% CI 0.954, 1.331) over time in the overall sample. There were no statistically significant interactions between diabetes and time in explaining changes in total brain volume (β −0.451; 95% CI −1.807, 0.905; p = 0.51) and ventricular volume (β 0.175; 95% CI −0.009, 0.359; p = 0.06). Three-way interaction terms of diabetes and time were not statistically significant with age, sex, ApoE4, baseline WMH volume, infarcts or microbleeds (p > 0.05). There were also no statistically significant associations when data were imputed (Table 3), in a worst case (Table 4) or best case (ESM Table 1) scenario.

Type 2 diabetes, brain atrophy and cognition

The addition of total brain or lateral ventricular volume did not alter the associations between type 2 diabetes and decline in verbal memory or fluency (results not shown).

Discussion

In older community-dwelling people without a history of dementia, type 2 diabetes was associated with reduced cognitive function at baseline and a greater decline in verbal memory and verbal fluency independently of confounding factors. Although type 2 diabetes was associated with lower total brain volume and higher ventricular volume at baseline, it was not associated with the rate of brain atrophy and atrophy did not mediate associations between type 2 diabetes and cognitive decline.

Type 2 diabetes and cognitive decline

We confirmed that type 2 diabetes was associated with poorer baseline cognitive function, suggesting an impact on cognitive reserve that may begin before older age. In addition, even over a relatively short period of ~5 years, we found that type 2 diabetes was associated with faster decline in verbal fluency (a measure of executive ability) and memory. For example, in people without type 2 diabetes, verbal fluency slightly increased on average each year (0.004 SD/units per year), whereas it declined at more than triple the rate in those with type 2 diabetes (−0.023 SD/units per year). Such accelerated cognitive decline may contribute to executive difficulties in everyday activities and health behaviours (such as medication compliance), which in turn may poorly influence future vascular health and cognitive decline, and possibly an earlier onset of dementia in those with type 2 diabetes [33]. Previous findings have been mixed, particularly in middle vs older age. Studies commencing in midlife and followed up over 10 years or more tend to show that type 2 diabetes is associated with faster decline in global cognitive function [3, 6], processing speed [4, 6], executive function [3, 4], verbal fluency [6] and memory [3, 4, 6]. However, in older cohorts, these associations are more variable, with some showing greater decline in global cognition [5, 34,35,36], but not others [8, 9, 37]. The non-significant results in our study for the global cognitive score may be explained by its derivation from a wider range of tests than used in other studies: some of the tests used showed less decline in type 2 diabetes (described further below) and hence a cancelling of effects. Studies in older age have also reported mixed results for specific cognitive domains: some have reported greater decline in verbal fluency [38] and memory [37, 39] in people with type 2 diabetes, whereas several others have reported no differences [5, 9, 34, 35, 38, 40]. These mixed findings most likely reflect differences in populations, follow-up times and cognitive measures. Surprisingly, we found that visuospatial function and visual memory were slower to decline in people with type 2 diabetes, although those with diabetes performed worse at baseline [11]. A previous study of people with (n = 68) and without type 2 diabetes (n = 38) also found a similar phenomenon, with healthy people showing a decline in visuospatial function (using a modified version of the Taylor Complex Figure), whereas people with diabetes tended to improve [41].

Type 2 diabetes and brain atrophy

Although we found that both ventricular and brain volume were worse at baseline in people with type 2 diabetes, there were no differences in rates of decline. Prior studies are very few and describe mixed results, some reporting type 2 diabetes to be associated with greater rates of increase in ventricular volume [16, 17], but not in decline of total brain volume over time [17, 18, 20]. The difference in baseline volumes in our study suggests that changes in brain atrophy may begin earlier in life, such as in midlife, and carry the potential for reduced brain reserve. Indeed, in a previous study, midlife type 2 diabetes (mean age 54, SD 9.0) was associated with a greater increase in temporal horn volume of the lateral ventricle, thought to be a marker of hippocampal and medial temporal atrophy [18]. The absence of a greater rate of brain atrophy related to diabetes in our sample may be explained partly by the relatively short duration of follow-up, as well as the low burden of cerebrovascular disease [34,35,36]. In a recent autopsy study of people with a larger burden of cerebrovascular disease than in our sample, the authors reported the combination of diabetes and cerebrovascular disease was associated with lower cognitive scores at the end of life compared with the presence of either one risk factor alone [36]. It is possible that our sample had a lower burden of cerebrovascular disease as a result of relatively good blood glucose and blood pressure control and the high prevalence of blood pressure- and lipid-lowering drug use. In the absence of good control of these factors, the greater accrual of cerebrovascular disease may impact on the rates of brain atrophy in diabetes. This theory remains to be tested.

Type 2 diabetes, brain atrophy and cognitive decline

Contrary to our hypotheses and results from cross-sectional studies [11, 13], total brain or lateral ventricular volume did not mediate associations between type 2 diabetes and cognitive decline. In our previous cross-sectional analyses, brain volume appeared to substantially mediate the association between type 2 diabetes and cognition [11]. No other studies have compared decline in both cognition and brain atrophy between people with and without type 2 diabetes together in the same study. In the Women’s Health Initiative, associations between type 2 diabetes and future cognitive function (measured only once) were slightly attenuated by adding grey and white matter and ischaemic volumes to the models [20]. In the Prospective Study of Pravastatin in the Elderly at Risk (PROSPER) randomised trial (age 70–82 years; 89 participants with type 2 diabetes and 438 control individuals), baseline lower brain volume was associated with greater decline in an immediate picture-learning task but not in the Stroop Test in people with type 2 diabetes [19]. Studies which allow for greater accrual of cerebrovascular disease may be more likely to reveal the mediating impact of brain structure on the diabetes–cognitive decline relationship in older age.

Strengths and limitations

There are some limitations in our study. It was carried out over a relatively short time frame, and differences between groups in cognition and brain volume may occur over longer periods or earlier in midlife [3, 6]. We had a homogenous sample in reference to race and were therefore unable to explore its effects, whereas prior studies have found stronger associations in African-Americans [5, 38]. As in other longitudinal studies some participants were lost to follow-up, which might have impeded the ability to detect a signal (internal validity). But we used mixed models to enable inclusion of individuals with incomplete follow-up, as well as carrying out three sensitivity analyses to explore its effect. The change in MRI scanners during the middle of the study might have introduced bias in structural brain measures, although we attempted to correct for this using data obtained from the same participant in both scanners. The mean HbA1c for people with type 2 diabetes was relatively low (mean 5.6%), which reduces the ability of the study to be generalisable to other populations (external validity). The self-reported method for dementia diagnosis might have influenced findings if there were differences in under-reporting between groups.

Strengths of our study include longitudinal measurement of a wide range of cognitive tests covering different cognitive domains in conjunction with serial brain MRI scans. We explored the independence of associations between type 2 diabetes and cognitive decline for several confounding factors and also examined for effect modification by key covariates such as ApoE4, age and sex.

Conclusion and area for future research

In this study, type 2 diabetes was associated with decline in verbal memory and fluency in older community-dwelling people without dementia over a period of ~5 years but not with MRI markers of brain atrophy. The effects of type 2 diabetes and poor metabolic health at midlife, and the impact of accrual of cerebrovascular lesions at older age, both deserve further study to inform preventative efforts against dementia.