Biofeedback-produced hemispheric asymmetry of slow cortical potentials and its behavioural effects

https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-8760(90)90069-PGet rights and content

Abstract

Two studies served to examine behavioural effects of slow cortical potentials (SPs). SPs were manipulated by means of a biofeedback procedure. The ability of human subjects to alter SPs differentially between the two hemispheres - specifically over the lateral aspects of teh central sulcus - was tested by providing feedback ofthe SP difference betwrrn C3 and C4. In Expt. I, 21 of the 45 subjects produced hemispheric asymmetries of more than 2 μV between C3 and C4 on an average after 80 trials of analogue, continuous and immediate feedback. In Expt. II, SP changes were fed back digitally at the end of each trial. Within 120 trials, 20 of the 48 subjects reached the criterion of a minimum 2-μV difference in SPs between C3 and C4 on the average. Average differentiation remained significantly below the SP differentiations achieved for continuous feedback. Trials with feedback were followed by ‘task’ trials without feedback, during which subjects were still requested to produce SP changes but also had to complete a task: Either sensorimotor tasks (Expt. I) or forced choice handedness tasks (Expt. II) were presented to evaluate behavioural consequences of hemispheric SP differences. In subjects achieving the required SP differentiation it affected the behavioural output in agreement with the known functions of the respective cortical area.

References (67)

  • M. Kutas et al.

    Event-related potential asymmetry during the reading of sentences

    J. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1988)
  • W. Lutzenberger et al.

    Biofeedback of slow cortical potentials and its effects on the performance in mental arithmetic tasks

    Biol. Psychol.

    (1982)
  • W. Lutzenberger et al.

    Asymmetry of brain potentials related to sensorimotor tasks

    Int. J. Psychophysiol.

    (1985)
  • R. Oldfield

    The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory

    Neuropsychologia

    (1971)
  • E. Altenmüller

    Hirnelektrische Korrelate der cerebralen Musikverarbeitung bein Menschen

    Eur. Arch. Psychiat. Neurol. Sci.

    (1986)
  • E. Altenmüller et al.

    Electrophysiological evidence of hemispheric dominance for language, calculation and music

    Pfügers Arch. Eur. J. Physiol.

    (1984)
  • E. Altenmüller et al.

    Electrophysiological evidence of right parietal dominance of visual-spatial processing tasks

  • H. Bauer

    Regulation of slow brain potentials affects task performance

  • N. Birbaumer et al.

    Imagery affects slow cortical potentials

    Int. J. Neurosci.

    (1988)
  • N. Birbaumer et al.

    Slow potentials at the cerebral cortex and behavior

    Physiol. Rev.

    (1989)
  • E. Blanchard et al.

    A simple feedback system for self-control of blood pressure

    Percept. Motor Skills

    (1974)
  • T. Budzynski et al.

    An instrument for producing deep muscle relaxation by means of analog information feedback

    J. Appl. Behav. Anal.

    (1969)
  • M.G.H. Coles

    Modern mind reading: psychophysiology and cognition

    Psychophysiology

    (1989)
  • M.G.H. Coles et al.

    Detecting Early Communication: Using measures of Movement-related Potentials to Illuminate Human Information Processing

    (1988)
  • M. Colgan

    Effects of binary and proportional feedback on bidirectional control of heart rate

    Psychophysiology

    (1977)
  • W. Connor et al.

    Cortical slow-wave and cardiac rate responses in stimulus orientation and reaction time conditions

    J. Exp. Psychol.

    (1969)
  • T. Elbert

    Biofeedback langsamer kortikaler Potentiale

    (1978)
  • T. Elbert

    Patterns of stimulus- and self-induced slow brain potentials — a sign of task specific preparation

    Human Neurobiol

    (1986)
  • T. Elbert et al.

    The influence of low-level transcortical DC-currents on response speed in humans

    Int. J. Neurosci.

    (1981)
  • T. Elbert et al.

    Threshold regulation — a key for the understanding of the combined dynamicsof EEG and the event-related potentials

    J. Psychophysiol.

    (1987)
  • A.W. Gaillard

    Slow Brain Potentials Preceding Task Performance

    (1978)
  • W. Gehring et al.

    Response priming and components of the event-related brain potential

    Psychophysiology

    (1986)
  • Cited by (54)

    • Electrophysiological CNS-processes related to associative learning in humans

      2016, Behavioural Brain Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Also, some trials without feedback from the rocket ship or the target score were placed in between the feedback trials and in these trails, the ability to self-regulate the SCP was maintained in spite of the lacking feedback. Reports on SCP conditioning obtained through feedback have been reviewed [297,343], and include cases in which subjects were trained to increase the negativity of SPC in one hemisphere while suppressing it in the other [344,345]. The possibility that SCP control could be mediated via eye movement artifacts or scalp skin conductance had been excluded in an early work on operantly trained positive or negative SCP shifts [346].

    • Connectivity-based neurofeedback: Dynamic causal modeling for real-time fMRI

      2013, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      Intuitively, one might assume that continuous feedback is superior over intermittent feedback, because reinforcement is more directly linked to efforts of the participant, because it provides more opportunities to evaluate training success, and because it keeps the task engagement high. In the field of EEG-based feedback, a few studies indeed reported that a shorter temporal contiguity facilitates learning (Rockstroh et al., 1990; Travis et al., 1974). However, because of the different temporal resolutions of EEG and fMRI, these findings might not be transferable.

    • Real-time fMRI and its application to neurofeedback

      2012, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      For example, it was shown that lateralized SCP regulation induced lateralized reaction time differences (Rockstroh et al., 1990). At the University of Tübingen, Niels Birbaumer and his lab had done pioneering work and established EEG feedback for various applications in both neuroscience and clinics (Birbaumer et al., 1999; Kotchoubey et al., 2001; Rockstroh et al., 1990). However, he realized that the lack of precise localization and limited coverage of EEG would severely limit the progress in neurofeedback and that fundamental methodological advances were overdue.

    • Computing moment-to-moment BOLD activation for real-time neurofeedback

      2011, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      Despite widespread interest, neurofeedback training based on fMRI has grown slowly in terms of number of publications, due at least partly to methodological challenges associated with data quality. Existing methods for real-time fMRI either do not compute moment-to-moment changes in activation (Cox et al., 1995; Yoo et al., 1999; Gembris et al., 2000), which is crucial in learning to control brain activation (Rockstroh et al., 1990), or provide a real-time neurofeedback signal (Goebel, 2001; deCharms et al., 2004) computed without accounting for the substantial noise corrupting fMRI data (Friston et al., 1994). Here we present a new method for computing fMRI-based neurofeedback that separates moment-to-moment changes in the fMRI signal intensity attributable to neural sources from those due to the non-random fMRI noise, resulting in a feedback signal more reflective of neural activation.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text