Elsevier

Acta Psychologica

Volume 100, Issues 1–2, November 1998, Pages 161-175
Acta Psychologica

The influence of mental and motor load on handwriting movements in Parkinsonian patients

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0001-6918(98)00032-8Get rights and content

Abstract

This experiment tested the hypothesis that Parkinson’s disease (PD) patients are more vulnerable to a moderate level of secondary task load than elderly or young controls due to heightened variability in the motor system. PD patients, elderly, and young adults performed a handwriting task with different secondary tasks. The secondary task imposed motor load (i.e., speech) and/or a mental load (i.e., ignoring, repeating, or subtracting). The findings showed that, in contrast to young and elderly controls, PD patients tended to increase MT, accumulated pause time, and normalized jerk when the secondary task consisted primarily of motor load. Furthermore, it was shown that PD patients did not reduce writing sizes as result of a high level of mental load which finding suggests that writing in an automated fashion does not result in micrographia. The results are discussed in relation to strategies imposed to contend with reduced signal-to-noise levels in the motor system.

Introduction

The handwriting of Parkinson’s disease (PD) patients is more impaired in terms of force amplitude than in terms of force timing when compared to handwriting of the elderly Teulings and Stelmach, 1991a, Teulings and Stelmach, 1991b, Teulings and Stelmach, 1992, Van Gemmert et al., 1998. As a result of impairments in force amplitude, PD patients experience problems with the fine tuning of forces Stelmach and Worringham, 1988, Stelmach et al., 1989, Kunesch et al., 1995 and often report muscle weakness (Shindo and Yanagawa, 1992). The elderly, in turn, make slower and spatially more variable movements as compared to young adults Stelmach et al., 1987, Stelmach et al., 1988, Seidler-Dobrin and Stelmach, 1998, but to a lesser extent than PD patients (Contreras-Vidal et.al., 1995).

It is suggested that slower and more variable movements in the elderly as compared to younger adults are due to reduced signal-to-noise ratios in the motor system of the elderly Welford, 1981, Walker et al., 1997. These impaired signal-to-noise ratios are probably caused by a combination of physiological aging processes, such as loss of motor units (Lexell et al., 1988), and reduced muscle strength as result of a reduction of the number of muscle fibers per muscle (Vandervoort, 1992). Additionally, it is suggested that PD patients have smaller signal-to-noise ratios than the elderly (cf. Teulings and Stelmach, 1991a). This account of movement slowness and variability is based on two assumptions about signal-to-noise ratios in the motor system. Firstly, the motor system is inherently noisy (Fitts, 1954). The noise in the motor system reflects the combined result of the stochastic recruitment process of muscle units due to characteristics of muscle tissue, physiological tremor, stretch reflexes, springlike oscillations of the limbs, and correction servos from feedback processing (Van Galen and Schomaker, 1992). Secondly, noise in the motor system increases with larger force levels resulting in an increase of the variability in peak force Sherwood and Schmidt, 1980, Stelmach et al., 1989, Van Galen and De Jong, 1995. Therefore, rapid movements are generally more noisy than slower movements, because fast movements require higher levels of force.

Recent studies by Van Gemmert and Van Galen, 1994, Van Gemmert and Van Galen, 1996, Van Gemmert and Van Galen, 1997, Van Gemmert et al., 1998 on the effects of different kinds of stress on point-to-point aiming and handwriting movements, suggested that both mental load and physical stress reduce signal-to-noise ratios. Furthermore, these studies showed that subjects can adapt relatively easy to moderate levels of mental load and physical stress. In these studies, it was argued that an increase of noise in the motor system as a consequence of mental load or physical stress is diminished by the filtering properties of the pen–limb system and/or the signal is enhanced by increasing processing time.

The latter view to increase signal-to-noise ratios in the motor system by increasing processing time is based on the notion that as a signal accrues over time, its noise levels off. This principle, which has a long history in detection theory (Tanner and Swets, 1954), leads to prolonged movement duration (i.e., longer movement times and/or pauses). If movement duration is to be kept constant, this adaptation strategy is not optional, because longer pause intervals need to be added between movements to allow for more processing time.

The first option to increase the signal-to-noise ratio in the motor system is to use the filtering properties of the effector system to actively cope with increased levels of neuromotor noise. To this end, subjects may enhance limb stiffness which then suppresses high-frequency components of the movement, these components are thought to produce more variable movement trajectory and end-point outcomes Ghez and Martin, 1982, Hogan, 1984, Van Galen and Schomaker, 1992, Van Galen et al., 1990. To some extent, the latter mechanism may increase signal-to-noise ratios without it needs to reduce movement speed. Active cocontraction of the agonist and antagonist muscle that lead to an increase of tonic limb stiffness is preferred over the detection-theory based option to increase signal-to-noise ratios, because this option requires longer movement durations. However the increase in cocontraction without loss of performance speed requires accurate modulations of muscle force (Seidler-Dobrin et al., in press). In the present experiment, the coping strategies of PD patients, young, and elderly control subjects will be investigated under various levels of mental and motor load by analyses of signal-to-noise ratios of handwriting movements.

Morris et al. (1995) suggested that movement performance of PD patients deteriorates when secondary cognitive or motor tasks are performed. Other studies Gnanalingham et al., 1997, Kuzis et al., 1997 failed to show cognitive deficits in mildly affected PD patients. These findings suggest that as a result of secondary cognitive tasks, neuromotor noise in the motor system of PD patients and elderly controls may be elevated to the same extent. Therefore, we argue that in mildly affected PD patients, a secondary cognitive task should deteriorate motor performance to the same extend as performance of the elderly, which will be less for a task consisting of moderate levels of cognitive load as compared to a task consists moderate levels of motor load. It is predicted that a deterioration of motor performance will be exhibited by a reduction of movement speed. Generally, a reduction in movement speed could either result from an increase in movement duration or a decrease in movement amplitude. Reduction of stroke size (i.e., movement amplitude) in handwriting is one of the characteristic clinical signs of PD often referred to as micrographia Sandyk, 1995, Sandyk and Iacono, 1994, Tetrud, 1991, Helsper et al., 1996. If PD patients would reduce their movement amplitude as a result of a secondary cognitive task (i.e., mental load), but not as result of a secondary motor task (i.e., motor load), this could explain the reduced writing size (micrographia) of PD patients, because handwriting in daily life often occurs in situations where mental load is present, like simultaneous calculations, and memory recall. Evidence supporting this view is given by Oliveira et al. (1997) who showed that PD patients with micrographia benefitted from external cues to increase their writing size. They suggested that the external cues encouraged PD patients to write less automatically. Therefore, secondary tasks that increase mental load during handwriting should reduce writing size, because the writing task must then be performed in a more automated fashion to free resources to fulfil the requirements of the secondary task. To investigate this explanation and test the hypothesis that motor performance of PD patients deteriorates during motor load, an experiment was designed in which a handwriting task was performed under three levels of mental load, induced by a secondary arithmetic task, and two levels of motor load, induced by a secondary speech task. There were three subject groups: PD patients, normal young adults, and normal elderly adults.

We expected that increased motor load would increase the neural noise in the PD patients as manifested by increased variability (jerk), longer movement durations, and reduced writing size. Furthermore, we expected that a high level of mental load would show a similar deterioration of motor performance for PD patients as for the elderly. Finally, we predicted that the higher level of mental load would result in more variable movements and longer movement durations for both the elderly and young adults, but that the movements of the elderly would be more affected than those of the young adults, because signal-to-noise ratios are generally reduced in the elderly due to aging processes.

Section snippets

Participants

Nine Parkinson’s disease (PD) patients between the ages of 52 and 72 years (five males and four females), nine healthy elderly individuals (elderly group) between the ages of 65 and 81 years (four males and five females), and nine healthy young adults (young group) between the ages of 21 and 27 years (four males and five females) volunteered to participate in the experiment. All individuals signed an informed consent form. For their participation, the PD patients and the elderly were paid. The

Movement time

The young adults required at average 1301 ms to write the word level, whereas the elderly needed 1583 ms, and PD patients took 1673 ms to complete this word (F(2,24)=6.07, p < 0.01). The REGW post hoc test determined that young adults were significantly faster (i.e., required at average less MT) than the elderly, who in turn were significantly faster than PD patients.

MTs for the different load conditions were 1603 ms, 1525 ms, 1456 ms, and 1491 ms, for the subtract, repeat, ignore, and control

Discussion

The aim of this study was to determine whether handwriting performance of PD patients is more vulnerable to increased motor load variations than performance of elderly and young adults. It was hypothesized that the signal-to-noise ratio in the motor system of PD patients are much smaller than signal-to-noise ratios of elderly controls which in PD patients contribute to their increased susceptibility to dual-tasks involving motor components. Furthermore, signal-to-noise ratios of the elderly

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by NINDS grant R01 NS 33173 awarded to George E. Stelmach. We thank the elderly, and the Parkinsonian patients of Arizona for their willingness to participate in this experiment. Furthermore, we would like to offer our gratitude to R.G.J. Meulenbroek for the many helpful comments on an earlier draft of this article.

References (54)

  • H. Bergman et al.

    Reversal of experimental parkinsonism by lesions of the subthalamic nucleus

    Science

    (1990)
  • J.L. Contreras-Vidal et al.

    Micrographia in Parkinson’s disease

    NeuroReport

    (1995)
  • Ferrandez, A.M., Durup, M., Farioli, F., 1996. Slowness, variability, and modulations of gait in healthy elderly. In:...
  • P.M. Fitts

    The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement

    Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (1954)
  • S. Fleminger

    Control of simultaneous movements distinguishes depressive motor retardation from Parkinson’s disease and neuroleptic Parkinsonism

    Brain

    (1992)
  • C. Ghez et al.

    The control of rapid limb movement in the cat

    III. Agonist-antagonist coupling. Experimental Brain Research

    (1982)
  • K.K. Gnanalingham et al.

    Motor and cognitive function in Lewy body dementia: Comparison with Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’s diseases

    Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry

    (1997)
  • Helsper, E., Teulings, H.L., Karamat, E., Stelmach, G.E., 1996. Preclinical Parkinson features in optically scanned...
  • M.M. Hoehn

    Commentary; Parkinsonism: Onset, progression, and mortality

    Neurology

    (1998)
  • M.M. Hoehn et al.

    Parkinsonism: Onset, progression, and mortality

    Neurology

    (1967)
  • N. Hogan

    Adaptive control of mechanical impedance by coactivation of antagonist muscles

    IEEE Transactions on Automatic Control

    (1984)
  • W. Hutchinson et al.

    Effects of apomorphine on globus pallidus neurons in Parkinsonian patients

    Annals of Neurology

    (1997)
  • G. Kuzis et al.

    Cognitive functions in major depression and Parkinson disease

    Archives of Neurology

    (1997)
  • Morris, M.E., Iansek, R., Summers, J.J., Matyas, T.A., 1995. Motor control considerations for the rehabilitation of...
  • R.M. Oliveira et al.

    Micrographia in Parkinson’s disease: The effect of providing external cues

    Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry

    (1997)
  • R. Sandyk

    Parkinsonian micrographia reversed by treatment with weak electromagnetic fields

    International Journal of Neuroscience

    (1995)
  • R. Sandyk et al.

    Reversal of micrographia in Parkinson’s disease by application of picoTesla range magnetic fields

    International Journal of Neuroscience

    (1994)
  • Cited by (55)

    • Semi-global Parameterization of Online Handwriting Features for Characterizing Early-Stage Alzheimer and Mild Cognitive Impairment

      2018, IRBM
      Citation Excerpt :

      Handwriting deterioration may be a sign of a dysfunction of the fine motor program, and an early symptom of a neurodegenerative disease. In the e-health field, online HW analysis has followed two main trends: the first consists in investigating generative models of HW [2–4], while the second concerns pathology detection or the quantitative assessment of therapeutic effects, by investigating discriminative HW parameters [5–16]. In the first trend, some motor models of HW take into account movements' kinematics and neuromuscular activity [2–4].

    • Personal digital bodyguards for e-security, e-learning and e-health: A prospective survey

      2018, Pattern Recognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      According to the lognormality principle, it is expected that some age-related problems will affect fine motor control subject fine motricity, causing performances to depart from an ideal lognormal behaviour. To give a few examples of pioneers as well as more recent papers, we can point out several studies dealing with Parkinson's disease [221–225], Alzheimer's disease [226], Huntington's disease [227], schizophrenia [228,229], cognitive impairment [230,231], hemispheric lesions [232], agraphia [233,234], dysgraphia [167,235,236], micrographia [237,238], macrographia [239], tardive dyskinesia [240], autism [241], dystonia and writer's cramp [242], visuospatial neglect [243,244], hyperactivity [245], attention problems [246], ADHD [247], depression [248], psychosis [249], alcohol consumption [250], nicotine [251], cannabis [252] and other illicit drugs [253]. Although some of these studies were not always rigorously supported, some reporting case studies, many being not computerized, their fundamental questions [254–256] were gradually transposed in the ‘80 and early ‘90, to investigations dealing with automatic on-line handwriting processing [257,258].1

    • How to design finger input of Chinese characters: A literature review

      2014, International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text