Attentional demands of continuously monitoring orientation using vestibular information

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00113-0Get rights and content

Abstract

The aim of this series of experiments was to determine whether attention is normally required for continuously processing vestibular information concerning orientation, or is required only when orientation is disrupted (eg by vestibular dysfunction or by conflicting visual and vestibular orientation cues). In the first two studies, healthy subjects were passively oscillated, and indicated when they perceived they were passing through their starting position. There was only weak evidence for interference between performance on this ‘continuous orientation monitoring task’ and on concurrent mental tasks. However, a third study showed that when patients with vestibular imbalance carried out the continuous orientation monitoring task their performance on a concurrent mental arithmetic task was substantially impaired. This dual task interference was correlated with inaccuracy in judging orientation on the continuous orientation monitoring task, which in turn correlated with severity of recent vestibular symptomatology (assessed by questionnaire). In a fourth experiment, disorientation was induced in healthy subjects by rotating the visual field about the line of sight. Bidirectional interference was observed between monitoring orientation (assessed by accuracy in setting a rod to the perceived vertical) and performance of an arithmetic task. Dual task interference was correlated with baseline levels of disorientation induced by the visual field, as indicated by inaccuracy in judging the visual vertical. These findings suggest that monitoring orientation makes significant demands upon cortical processing resources when disorientation is induced, whether the disorientation results from deficient sensory functioning or from ambiguous perceptual information.

Introduction

In order to coordinate our physical activity and postural control with respect to the environment, we continuously monitor and update our position and motion in space by means of central integration of information from the visual, somatosensory and vestibular senses. However, it has been shown that vestibular information alone can be utilised to update representations of position in space, which have been assessed by passively rotating vision- deprived subjects and then requiring them either to report their position verbally [5], [19], [26], to reposition themselves to their original orientation [10], [17], [31], [36], or to orient their eyes towards the remembered location of a visual target presented before they were moved [4], [18].

The cortical structures that are involved in the central processing of vestibular information for orientation and navigation have been investigated by means of neuroimaging [7], [23], [35] and studies of patients with cortical lesions [16], [20], [33]. While it seems evident that the parieto-insular cortex plays a crucial role in spatial processing, including the processing of vestibular information [8], additional areas are known to make an important contribution to spatial representation. These include the hippocampus [14], [35], accessed from the parieto-insular cortex or from subcortical structures, as well as head direction cell systems [3].

Spatial updating has been characterised as an ‘automatic’ process, in the sense that it occurs involuntarily, and cannot be suppressed without mental effort, when forming a spatial representation [30]. However, it may not be ‘automatic’ in the sense of making no demands on attentional capacity [30], [24]. The dual task technique provides a means to examine the extent to which different activities demand attentional capacity, ie draw on common central processing resources. In dual task experiments, decrement in performance occurring when two tasks are performed simultaneously (relative to baseline performance on each single task) indicates that both tasks demand attention. The nature of that common resource might be general, for example, the two tasks might depend upon ‘central executive’ control which is needed for coordinating multiple task performance [2]. Alternatively, the resource the two tasks share may be more specific; for example, if both tasks involve spatial processing then they might compete for specific spatial processing resources [25]. The advantage of using dual task experimentation in healthy subjects to complement studies of patients with cortical lesions is that adaptation following injury may result in idiosyncratic alterations in central processing.

There have been remarkably few dual task studies of the attentional processes involved in monitoring orientation using vestibular information. However, it has been shown that counting backwards can interfere with orientation when walking without vision [6], [32], a task which involves the use of both vestibular and non-vestibular information [28]. Moreover, Yardley and colleagues have demonstrated interference between healthy subjects’ performance on tasks involving purely vestibular perception of orientation and mental tasks (ie reaction time to an auditory stimulus, counting backwards) [39], [40], [42].

The results of Yardley and colleagues were interpreted as indicating that attention is required for vestibular perception of orientation; ie vestibular information depends upon central mental processing, and suffers from capacity limits. However, in all of these studies subjects were required to indicate their orientation after being passively moved. The advantage of this methodology was that the mental task was performed during self-motion but the orientation response was carried out after the mental task and self-motion had been completed; thus interference could not have simply resulted from concurrent performance of two responses (ie responses to the orientation and mental tasks). But in order to perform such a task the participant does not simply process sensory orientation information—in order to utilise this information to generate an accurate response after the self-motion task is completed, the information must also be stored as a representation of orientation in memory. Therefore it remains possible that attention was only required in order to encode or maintain a spatial representation in memory, but not for processing vestibular information online.

In order to examine whether attention is required for online processing of vestibular information concerning orientation, a new task was devised. For our new ‘continuous orientation monitoring’ task, subjects were continuously rotated in a motorised chair through a pseudo-random series of brief turns of varying amplitudes and alternating direction. Subjects were required to press a button whenever they judged themselves to be facing a reference point at a fixed location within the room (ie their initial starting position or ‘origin’), and therefore had to continuously update their perceived spatial position relative to the origin. To establish the accuracy of continuously monitoring orientation using only vestibular information, an initial pilot experiment was carried out to compare responses in the light (when visual information could be used) and in darkness (when only vestibular information was available).

A series of experiments in healthy subjects and patients were then used to investigate whether continuously monitoring orientation normally demands attention, or whether it demands attention only when deficits in monitoring are induced, either by vestibular dysfunction, or by experimental manipulation of perceptual conditions. If processing sensory information in order to continuously monitor orientation can normally be accomplished with little or no requirement for attention, healthy subjects’ performance on the continuous orientation monitoring task should be unaffected by a requirement to simultaneously carry out a mental task. In the first two studies we therefore compared healthy subjects’ accuracy in monitoring orientation with and without the requirement to carry out a concurrent mental task. To test the hypothesis that monitoring orientation does require attention when orientation is disrupted by sensory deficits or ambiguous perceptual information, the third study compared the single and dual task performance of patients with unilateral vestibular dysfunction, and determined the relationship between symptoms and task performance, while the final study examined the effect of experimentally induced disorientation on concurrent performance of an orientation and an arithmetic task in healthy subjects.

Section snippets

Experiment 1. Concurrent orientation and auditory monitoring tasks: healthy subjects

The aim of this study was to determine whether performing a concurrent attention-demanding auditory monitoring task affects the accuracy of healthy subjects on a continuous orientation monitoring task, assessed in terms of constant and variable error in orientation judgements.

Experiment 2. Concurrent orientation and arithmetic tasks: healthy subjects

The main aim of this study was to determine whether the increase in constant error on the continuous orientation monitoring task induced by monitoring auditory stimuli in the first experiment was an artefact of the timing requirements of concurrently performing these tasks. We therefore examined whether performing a self-paced task also affects the accuracy of healthy subjects on the continuous orientation monitoring task (assessed in terms of constant and variable error in orientation

Experiment 3. Concurrent orientation and arithmetic tasks: subjects with vestibular dysfunction

The aim of this study was to determine whether concurrently performing the arithmetic and continuous orientation monitoring tasks used in the second experiment affects the performance of subjects with vestibular dysfunction (assessed in terms of constant and variable error in orientation judgements, and number of correct subtractions on the arithmetic task, relative to the baseline single task condition). Our rationale was that patients with disordered vestibular functioning may have to devote

Experiment 4. Concurrent orientation and arithmetic tasks: induction of disorientation in healthy subjects

The aim of this experiment was to determine whether concurrently performing an arithmetic and a continuous orientation monitoring task affects the performance of healthy subjects when they are disoriented by a rotating visual display (where performance is assessed in terms of constant and variable error in orientation judgements, and number of correct subtractions on the arithmetic task, relative to the baseline single task condition). Experimental induction of disorientation was employed in

Conclusions

This series of experiments provided only very weak evidence that monitoring rotation using vestibular information demands attention in healthy subjects who are not disoriented. It is therefore possible that the attentional demands of studies in which delayed orientation responses were required [39], [40] were principally related to the necessity to encode or maintain a representation of orientation in memory. There was stronger evidence that individuals who were disoriented, whether because of

Acknowledgements

The first three experiments were supported by grant number 048013 from the Wellcome Trust and the final experiment was funded by a CEC Transnational Access to Major Research Infrastructures grant. All experiments were carried out at the MRC Human Movement and Balance Unit, Section of Neuro-otology, Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery, Queen Square, London, which supported Michael Gresty and Adolfo Bronstein, and provided testing facilities and technical support. We thank David Buckwell for

References (42)

  • J. Blouin et al.

    Perception of passive whole-body rotations in the absence of neck and body proprioception

    Journal of Neurophysiology

    (1995)
  • A. Böök et al.

    Processing of information about location during locomotion: effects of a concurrent task and locomotion patterns

    Scandinavian Journal of Psychology

    (1980)
  • G. Bottini et al.

    Identification of the central vestibular projections in man: a positron emission tomography activation study

    Experimental Brain Research

    (1994)
  • T. Brandt et al.

    The vestibular cortex: its locations, functions, and disorders

    Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences

    (1999)
  • G.B. Brookes et al.

    Recovery from unilateral vestibular nerve section in human subjects evaluated by physiological, psychological and questionnaire assessments

    Acta Otolaryngologica (Stockholm)

    (1994)
  • M.A. Brookes et al.

    Sensing and controlling rotational orientation in normal subjects and patients with loss of labyrinthine function

    American Journal of Otology

    (1993)
  • J. Cohen

    Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences

    (1988)
  • C.L. Darlington et al.

    Further evidence for gender differences in circularvection

    Journal of Vestibular Research

    (1998)
  • J. Dichgans et al.

    Moving visual scenes influence the apparent direction of gravity

    Science

    (1972)
  • O. Ghaem et al.

    Mental navigation along memorized routes activates the hippocampus, precuneus, and insula

    Neuroreport

    (1997)
  • I. Israël et al.

    Spatial memory and path integration studied by self-driven passive linear displacement. 1. Basic properties

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (1997)
  • Cited by (53)

    • Vestibular and postural impairment in congenital Cytomegalovirus infection

      2022, International Journal of Pediatric Otorhinolaryngology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Episodic vertigo with neurovegetative symptoms has been previously reported in cCMV patients, and particularly attributed to the development of delayed endolymphatic hydrops [36–38]. Furthermore, vestibular impairment in cCMV has been linked also to cognitive dysfunctions, such as attention disorders (learning, reading, and writing disorders) and memory alterations, even if both the neurologic and visual dysfunction in cCMV children may be co-responsible for these cognitive sequelae [45–50]. The distinction of delayed or unachieved motor milestones from a progressive impairment of locomotor abilities should be always ruled out [40].

    • Vestibular Evaluation and Management of Children with Sensorineural Hearing Loss

      2021, Otolaryngologic Clinics of North America
      Citation Excerpt :

      Compensation for VD demands use of cognitive resources to accomplish basic tasks such as staying upright or stabilizing vision. Because maintaining postural stability is a priority, spatial and nonspatial tasks are equally impacted by the reduction in cognitive resources created by VD.58 The use of cognitive resources to maintain balance contributes to fatigue and takes away from the availability of these resources to perform other tasks such as conversing or reading.46

    • Impaired math achievement in patients with acute vestibular neuritis

      2017, Neuropsychologia
      Citation Excerpt :

      Our findings of impaired math performance are consistent with the frequent subjective complaints of patients with PVD (Harun et al., 2015). They are also in line with the existing, yet scarce empirical evidence of impaired numerical cognition in vestibular patients (Andersson et al., 2003; Bessot et al., 2012; Risey and Briner, 1990; Yardley et al., 2002). Unlike previous studies, we demonstrated impaired arithmetic performance under static conditions.

    • The Vestibular System

      2015, The Rat Nervous System: Fourth Edition
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text