NEUROPSYCHOLOGY OF SUBCORTICAL DEMENTIAS

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0193-953X(05)70352-XGet rights and content

The term s ubcortical dementia was first used to describe a distinct clinical syndrome in a 1974 literature review and case report of five patients with progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP). Albert et al1 described subcortical dementias as sharing a characteristic configuration of deficits, including “forgetfulness,” slowed thought processes (bradyphrenia), psychiatric changes (especially apathy, depression, and irritability), and impaired ability to manipulate acquired knowledge. They distinguished this pattern of deficits from the one seen in “cortical dementias,” such as Alzheimer's disease (AD), which includes anterograde amnesia, aphasia, agnosia, and apraxia. This term has since been broadened to encompass a wide range of disorders sharing primary pathology in subcortical structures, such as the basal ganglia, brain stem, thalamus, and central white matter—including such heterogeneous disorders as Parkinson's disease (PD); Huntington's disease (HD); multiple system atrophy; idiopathic basal ganglia calcification; multi-infarct dementia; and, most recently, AIDS dementia complex.24, 27, 105

This article describes the neurobiologic and cognitive features of three prototypical subcortical dementias: Parkinson's disease (PD), Huntington's disease (HD), and progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP). Although many others could be covered here, these three have been tied most closely to the concept of subcortical dementia. The article also summarizes common neuropsychologic features shared by subcortical dementias, overviews the anatomic and functional organization of frontal-striatal systems, considers the validity of using “subcortical dementia” as a classification label, and discusses clinical implications for assessing these patients.

Section snippets

PARKINSON'S DISEASE

Idiopathic PD is a common neurologic disorder, characterized by resting tremor, rigidity (especially “cogwheel”), akinesia, and postural as well as gait abnormalities. In addition, psychiatric disturbances are common, especially depression, which may predate neuropsychological symptoms. Some PD groups show more significant decline in cognitive abilities than others, which has led investigators to distinguish between “demented” and “nondemented” variants. In addition, there is evidence that some

HUNTINGTON'S DISEASE

Huntington's disease (HD) is an inherited progressive neurologic disease characterized by choreiform (involuntary spasmodic) and athetotic (writhing) movements. Psychiatric symptoms, including depression and psychosis, are also common and may precede motor manifestations of the disorder.24 In contrast to PD, HD invariably leads to the development of generalized dementia; thus, HD strictly conforms to characteristics of subcortical dementia only during the early to middle stages, prior to the

PROGRESSIVE SUPRANUCLEAR PALSY

PSP is also called Steele-Richardson-Olszewski syndrome, after the investigators who first defined it as a distinct clinical syndrome in 1964.110 It is also the primary disorder on which Albert and colleagues1 originally based the concept of “subcortical dementia.” Albert et al1 described PSP patients as having characteristic “forgetfulness,” slowed cognitive processing, psychiatric changes, and problems manipulating acquired knowledge. In addition to cognitive problems, clinical motor features

SHARED NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL FEATURES OF SUBCORTICAL DEMENTIAS

Although PD, HD, and PSP groups share a number of deficits, not all are useful in differentiating them from the so-called cortical dementias, such as AD. For instance, both subcortical dementias and AD commonly show abnormalities in visuospatial processes,90 and performance on immediate and delayed free recall memory tests is often similar.89 The specific pattern of memory and executive dysfunction, however, does differ and this provides a means to functionally segregate cortical and

ANATOMIC AND FUNCTIONAL ORGANIZATION OF THE FRONTAL-STRIATAL SYSTEM

The past years have witnessed a tremendous growth in knowledge of the structural and functional interconnectivity between the basal ganglia and cortex.3, 126 A basic grasp of the organization of these networks is critical to understanding the impact of brain dysfunction on cognition in subcortical dementias (see elsewhere in this issue). The cortical—basal ganglia connections are grouped into a number of segregated parallel circuits that originate with projections from widespread regions of the

VALIDITY OF “SUBCORTICAL DEMENTIA” AS A CLASSIFICATION LABEL

Some discussion is necessary regarding the weaknesses and strengths of “subcortical dementia” as a clinical classification label, as its use has been challenged on a number of grounds. For instance, close analysis of anatomic changes in dementia indicate that the term “subcortical” is probably too general.124 Subcortical dementia groups have cortical involvement in the frontal cortex and possibly even in the medial temporal cortex.59 They also have significant loss of ascending cholinergic

CLINICAL CONSIDERATIONS

The value of the label “subcortical dementia” probably comes primarily from its ability to accurately differentiate groups of patients in clinically useful ways. Although it certainly cannot account for the inherent complexity of these diverse disorders, consideration of certain key features can provide an effective means of classifying cognitive dysfunction. It can be beneficial to determine if dementia is more characteristic of a subcortical profile or more similar to an AD-type dementia.

CONCLUSION

“Subcortical dementias” are a heterogeneous group of disorders that share the relatively specific disruption of frontal-striatal system functions. This article discussed three prototypical subcortical dementias—Parkinson's disease, Huntington's disease, and progressive supranuclear palsy. Although there are important cognitive differences among these disorders, they share certain characteristic features that are useful in discriminating them from the so-called cortical dementias, such as

References (127)

  • G.E. Alexander et al.

    Basal ganglia-thalamocortical circuits: Parallel substrates for motor, oculomotor, “prefrontal” and “limbic” functions

    Prog Brain Res

    (1990)
  • E.H. Alyward et al.

    Reduced basal ganglia volume associated with the gene for Huntington's disease in asymptomatic at-risk persons

    Neurology

    (1994)
  • A. Antonini et al.

    Striatal glucose metabolism and dopamine D2 receptor binding in asymptomatic gene carriers and patients with Huntington's disease

    Brain

    (1996)
  • H. Appollonio et al.

    Implicit and explicit memory in patients with Parkinson's disease and without dementia

    Arch Neurol

    (1994)
  • K.A. Bamford et al.

    Clinical-pathologic correlation in Huntington's disease: A neuropsychological and computed tomography study

    Neurology

    (1989)
  • W.W. Beatty et al.

    Picture and motor sequencing in Parkinson's disease

    J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol

    (1990)
  • S. Berent et al.

    Positron emission tomographic scan investigations of Huntington's disease: Cerebral metabolic correlates of cognitive function

    Ann Neurol

    (1988)
  • F. Boller et al.

    Visuospatial impairment in Parkinson's disease: Role of perceptual and motor factors

    Arch Neurol

    (1984)
  • M.W. Bondi et al.

    Implicit and explicit memory in Alzheimer's disease and Parkinson's disease

    J Clin Exp Neuropsychol

    (1991)
  • M.W. Bondi et al.

    Contributions of frontal system dysfunction to memory and perceptual abilities in Parkinson's disease

    Neuropsychology

    (1993)
  • D.J. Brooks

    Detection of preclinical Parkinson's disease with PET

    Neurology

    (1991)
  • D.J. Brooks et al.

    The relationship between locomotor disability, autonomic dysfunction, and the integrity of the striatal dopaminergic system in patients with multiple system atrophy, pure autonomic failure, and Parkinson's disease, studied with PET

    Brain

    (1990)
  • R.G. Brown et al.

    Dual task performance and processing resources in normal subjects and patients with Parkinson's disease

    Brain

    (1991)
  • T.J. Brozoski et al.

    Cognitive deficit caused by regional depletion of dopamine in prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkey

    Science

    (1979)
  • N. Butters et al.

    Dissociation of implicit memory in dementia: Neurological implications

    Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society

    (1990)
  • N. Butters et al.

    Comparison of the neuropsychological deficits associated with early and advanced Huntington's disease

    Arch Neurol

    (1978)
  • N. Butters et al.

    Differentiation of amnesic and demented patients with the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised

    Clin Neuropsychol

    (1988)
  • A.S. Chan et al.

    Comparison of the semantic networks in patients with dementia and amnesia

    Neuropsychology

    (1985)
  • J.L. Cummings

    Frontal-subcortical circuits and human behavior

    Arch Neurol

    (1993)
  • J.L. Cummings et al.

    Subcortical dementia: Review of an emerging concept

    Arch Neurol

    (1984)
  • J.L. Cummings et al.

    Visuospatial abnormalities in Parkinson's disease

  • R. D'Antona et al.

    Subcortical dementia: Frontal cortex hypometabolism detected by positron emission tomography in patients with progressive supranuclear palsy

    Brain

    (1985)
  • I. Daum et al.

    Semantic memory impairment in Alzhheimer's disease

    J Clin Exp Neuropsychol

    (1996)
  • S.M. De la Monte et al.

    Neuropathological distinction between Parkinson's dementia and Parkinson's plus Alzheimer's disease

    Ann Neurol

    (1989)
  • D.C. Dellis et al.

    California Verbal Learning Test

    (1987)
  • Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders

    (1994)
  • B. Dubois et al.

    Biochemical correlates of cognitive changes and dementia in Parkinson's disease

  • B. Dubois et al.

    Slowing of cognitive processing in progressive supranuclear palsy: A comparison with Parkinson's disease

    Arch Neurol

    (1988)
  • K.A. Flowers et al.

    Recognition memory in Parkinson's disease

    Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry

    (1984)
  • N.L. Foster et al.

    Cerebral hypometabolism in progressive supranuclear palsy studied with positron emission tomography

    Ann Neurol

    (1988)
  • Freedman M: Parkinson's disease. In Cummings (ed): Subcortical Dementias. 1990, pp...
  • B. Giordani et al.

    Longitudinal neuropsychological and genetic linkage analysis of persons at risk for Huntington's disease

    Arch Neurol

    (1995)
  • S. Goto et al.

    Subdivisional involvment of nigrostriatal loop in idopathic Parkinson's disease and striatonigral degeneration

    Ann Neurol

    (1989)
  • J. Grafman et al.

    Frontal lobe functions in progressive supranuclear palsy

    Ann Neurol

    (1990)
  • S.T. Grafton et al.

    A comparison of neurological, metabolic, structural, and genetic evaluations in persons at risk for Huntington's disease

    Ann Neurol

    (1990)
  • S.T. Grafton et al.

    Serial changes of cerebral glucose metabolism and caudate size in persons at risk for Huntington's disease

    Arch Neurol

    (1992)
  • A.M. Graybiel et al.

    The nigrostriatal system in Parkinson's disease

    Adv Neurol

    (1990)
  • M. Grossman et al.

    Visual construction impairments in Parkinson's disease

    Neuropsychology

    (1993)
  • D.L. Harrington et al.

    Sequencing in Parkinson's disease: Abnormalities in programming and controlling movement

    Brain

    (1991)
  • D.L. Harrington et al.

    Procedural memory in Parkinson's disease: Impaired motor but not visuoperceptual learning

    J Clin Exp Neuropsychol

    (1990)
  • Cited by (61)

    • Study of neuropsychological deficits in late onset depression

      2020, Asian Journal of Psychiatry
      Citation Excerpt :

      Evidence on neuropsychological profile of sub-cortical cognitive impairment comes from sub-cortical dementia such as Parkinson’s disease, progressive supranuclear palsy and vascular dementia. In these conditions the cognitive impairment is characterised by impaired attention, executive dysfunction and relatively intact recognition and logical memory (Salmon and Filoteo, 2007; Savage, 1997). Studies on neurobiology of depression reported disruption of fronto-subcortical circuits as one of the hypothesis (Alexopoulos et al., 1997).

    • Prospective memory in obsessive compulsive disorder

      2018, Psychiatry Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Thus, the deficit appears to be of a higher order and executive in character with the obvious involvement of the strategic aspects of intention encoding (use of organizational strategies such as semantic processing). Deficits in the strategic aspects of memory, which have close association with the executive processes and robust implication for successful recall of information, is a well-known feature of disorders involving fronto-striatal dysfunction (Savage, 1997; Savage et al., 1999; Gershberg and Shimamura, 1995). In fact, research has shown that memory impairment in OCD is only secondary to executive dysfunction.

    • Corpus Striatum

      2014, Encyclopedia of the Neurological Sciences
    • Switching strategy underlies phonemic verbal fluency impairment in obsessive-compulsive disorder

      2012, Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      Previous neuropsychological studies have revealed deficits in various executive functions including mental-set shifting assessed by Trail-Making Test (Moritz et al., 2002; Moritz, Jelinek, Hottenrott, Klinge, & Randjbar, 2009; Schmidtke et al., 1998) and task switching evaluated by Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (Bannon et al., 2006; Greisberg & McKay, 2003; Okasha et al., 2000). Moreover, several neuropsychological studies have demonstrated that executive dysfunction contributes to impairment in cognitive function, such as verbal memory (Olley, Malhi, & Sachdev, 2007), nonverbal memory (Penadés et al., 2005; Savage et al., 1999), verbal learning (Deckersbach et al., 2005), and visuospatial abilities (Savage, 1997), suggesting the primary deficits in OCD may be impaired executive strategies, which have secondary effects on verbal or nonverbal ability. Recent neuroimaging and cognitive studies of OCD have reported the involvement of frontal-subcortical circuitry in the performance of various cognitive tasks, such as task-switching (Gu et al., 2007), and set-shifting (Watkins et al., 2005).

    • Neurodegenerative Disorders

      2011, Problem Solving in Neuroradiology
    • Huntington's disease

      2011, Handbook of Clinical Neurology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Impairment in executive memory has wide-ranging implications not only on cognitive assessments, but also on practical functions in everyday life. HD patients demonstrate impairment on various standard executive function tests, including the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, the Stroop Color Word Test, and the Tower of London (Paulsen et al., 1996a; Savage, 1997; Watkins et al., 2000). In fact, brief tests of executive function have shown utility as tools for differential diagnosis.

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    Address reprint requests to Cary Savage, PhD, Department of Psychiatry, Cognitive Neuroscience Group, 149–9102, Massachusetts General Hospital, Building 149, 13th Street, Charlestown, MA 02129

    This work was supported in part by a grant from the National Institute of Mental Health (01230).

    *

    From the Cognitive Neuroscience Group and Biostatistics, Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown; and the Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts

    View full text