Article Text

Download PDFPDF

Cognitive functioning after pallidotomy for refractory Parkinson’s disease
  1. Kenneth Perrinea,
  2. Michael Dogalib,
  3. Enrico Fazzinia,
  4. Djorje Sterioa,
  5. Edwin Kolodnya,
  6. David Eidelbergc,
  7. Orrin Devinskya,
  8. Aleksandar Berica
  1. aDepartment of Neurology, NYU School of Medicine and Hospital for Joint Diseases, New York, USA, bDepartment of Neurosurgery, University of California, Irvine, CA, USA, cDepartment of Neurology, Cornell University Medical Center and North Shore University Hospital, USA
  1. Dr Kenneth Perrine, Department of Neurology, Hospital for Joint Diseases, 301 East 17th Street, New York, NY 10003, USA. Telephone 001 212 598 6558; fax 001 212 598 6169.

Abstract

BACKGROUND Earlier approaches to pallidotomy for refractory Parkinson’s disease had significant complication rates. More recent approaches show fewer complications, but the effect of pallidotomy on cognition is unclear. The current study was conducted to examine the neuropsychological effects of unilateral pallidotomy.

METHODS Neuropsychological testing was performed on patients with medically refractory, predominantly unilateral Parkinson’s disease at baseline and after unilateral ventral pallidotomy (n=28) or after an equivalent period without surgery in control patients (n=10).

RESULTS Pallidotomy patients showed no significant changes from baseline to retesting relative to the control group for any measure. Across all of the tests administered, only five of the surgery patients showed a significant decline, and of these five none declined on more than one test. Depression did not relate to preoperative or postoperative cognition. The pallidotomy group showed a significant improvement in motor functioning and activities of daily living whereas the control group did not. These measures were not associated with the neuropsychological test scores at baseline or retest.

CONCLUSIONS Stereotactic unilateral ventral pallidotomy does not seem to produce dramatic cognitive declines in most patients.

  • Parkinson’s disease
  • pallidotomy
  • neuropsychological tests

Statistics from Altmetric.com

Request Permissions

If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.

Parkinson’s disease can be treated successfully with levodopa. However, some patients become refractory to dopaminergic treatment,1 and may be severely disabled from levodopa induced dyskinesias. Thalamotomy and pallidotomy can help to reduce the rigidity and tremor in Parkinson’s disease as well as the dyskinesias arising from treatment.2 Early pallidotomies often had significant complications but now carry low morbidity and mortality rates, although the effect of these ablations on cognitive functioning remains unclear.

Neuropsychological impairment may occur in the course of Parkinson’s disease. Although many patients experience little to no deficit, others develop a dementia3-6 or discrete deficits in visuospatial ability,7-9 memory,10-12 and frontal functions13-15 independent of the primary motor deficit. Patients with left body side signs (and presumably right basal ganglia impairment) may show more impaired cognitive functioning.6 16-20 Patients with right body side signs may show more impairment for verbal material and those with left body side signs may be more impaired for visuospatial material.16 18 21-25 The effect of these lateralised differences and material specificity on postoperative cognitive status is not known.

Cognitive changes after pallidotomy could be expected to occur in some functions based on prior lesion studies of the globus pallidus and the anatomical connections of the basal ganglia with cortical regions involved in cognition. Lesions to the globus pallidus can produce impaired attentional abilities and reductions in planning and organisation.26 The globus pallidus has primary efferents to the thalamus and through the thalamus to primary and supplementary motor cortex27 and to limbic regions.28Surgical lesions to the globus pallidus could thus impact on the initiation functions mediated by the supplementary motor cortex and the various cognitive functions affected by limbic cortex.

The current study examines cognitive functioning before and after unilateral stereotactic pallidotomy for refractory Parkinson’s disease. Patients who have undergone pallidotomy are compared with a comparable group of non-operated patients with Parkinson’s disease.

Methods

PATIENT SELECTION

Thirty eight patients with refractory Parkinson’s disease were recruited after protocol approval by the Institutional Review Boards of the Hospital for Joint Diseases and NYU Medical Center. All eligible patients had disabling fluctuations in motor functioning despite optimal medication management. Inclusion required a diagnosis of parkinsonism with a response to levodopa, the presence of bradykinesia and rigidity, and fluctuations between “off” parkinson symptoms and “on” dyskinesias. Excluded patients had known causes of parkinsonism (for example, encephalitis or neuroleptic treatment), focal brain abnormality on MRI, dementia on mental status testing, supranuclear gaze palsy, ataxia, orthostatic hypotension (blood pressure drop >30 mm Hg on standing), or coexisting illnesses that would complicate assessment or treatment (for example, spinal stenosis, cerebrovascular disease, metabolic disorders).

Twenty eight patients underwent stereotactic ventral pallidotomy (15 men, 13 women; 14 left side, 14 right side). Ten other patients (eight men, two women) were interested in pallidotomy but did not wish at the time of their initial evaluation to undergo surgery. These patients chose to forego surgery primarily because of lack of knowledge concerning the long term outcome of pallidotomy, and served as unoperated controls (three with left side symptoms, seven with right side symptoms). The surgery group did not differ significantly from the control group for age (combined mean 60.1 (SD 10.0) years), duration of Parkinson’s disease (combined mean 13.2 (SD 4.9 years)), levodopa medication dosage at first evaluation (combined mean 825.0 mg), or side of symptoms.

EVALUATIONS

All patients underwent at least two comprehensive evaluations. Details of the examinations were presented previously.29The first baseline evaluations were before surgery in the operative group. Subsequent evaluations were between 3 and 12 months later (postoperative for the surgery group, repeated testing without surgery for the control group). Data for each follow up evaluation were not available for all patients. Each evaluation consisted of neurological examination, including videotaped quantification of “on” and “off” states and motor functioning by adaptations of the Core Assessment Program for Intracerebral Transplantation (CAPIT)30 and Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS).31

Neuropsychological testing was conducted during the “on” state, with no data collected during “off” states, to minimise the deleterious effects of bradykinesia and bradyphrenia. Although dyskinesias were present in many patients during the neuropsychological testing, they were generally not sufficiently severe as to interfere with the tasks administered. Tests requiring a motor response (except for the trail making test) were not included in the battery to minimise the effect of these dyskinesias on the results.

OPERATIVE PROCEDURE

A Leksell stereotactic G-frame was placed and the centre aligned with the midpoint of the anterior commissural-posterior commissural (AC-PC) line. Axial MRI slices at 3 mm intervals were acquired inferior to the AC-PC line. The MRI images were digitised and transferred to a workstation (Cass/Midco, San Diego, CA, USA). The medial ventral global pallidus target contralateral to the body side with the most prominent symptoms was identified. The target was between 17 to 25 mm lateral to the midline, 6 to 8 mm inferior to the AC-PC line, and 2 to 3 mm anterior to the midcommissural point. A 3 mm twist drill was made in the same plane as the x coordinate lateral to the midline at an angle of 28° to 50° to the AC-PC line. The global pallidus target was verified by intraoperative electrical stimulation, impedance measurements, and single cell recordings.32 A lesioning electrode was then introduced using stereotactic coordinates from the computerised scan images. The electrode produced a cylindrical lesion consisting of multiple (three to six) overlapping lesions at 2 mm intervals by heating the tip to 80°C for 60 seconds. Lesion localisation in the global pallidus was confirmed by postoperative MRI (figure).

Axial, T1 Weighted MRI showing postoperative lesion in the right globus pallidus.

MEASURES

The activities of daily living (ADL) and motor scores from the UPDRS were selected as measures of preoperative and postoperative motor functioning. Scores from the neuropsychological battery administered preoperatively were compared with those on follow up testing. Pallidotomy patients had more frequent follow up testing at longer intervals than unoperated controls due to difficulties inherent in recruiting the unoperated group for multiple follow up assessments. Consequently, scores from more recent follow up testing for pallidotomy patients and from later follow up testing for unoperated controls were selected for comparison. However, even with this selection, the unoperated control patients had significantly shorter (mean = 4.5 (SD 1.6 months)) follow up neuropsychological testing than pallidotomy patients (mean 8.3 (SD 3.6 months; t=3.25, p<0.003)). To control for this difference, months between initial and follow up testing was covaried in statistical tests. Five of the 24 pallidotomy patients had neuropsychological testing before the follow up visit used for data in this study.

The neuropsychological battery was based on previous research on Parkinson’s disease,3-15 with the tests chosen on the basis of their sensitivity to the disease. The battery assessed overall mental status with the mini mental state examination.33Language was assessed by the controlled oral word association test34 to quantify verbal fluency. Visual scanning, motor speed, and cognitive flexibility were measured by part A and part B of the trail making test.35 Sustained concentration and cognitive processing speed without a motor component were assessed by the oral version of the symbol digit modalities test36 and by the Stroop test37 scored for the word, colour, and colour-word sections. Concept formation and perseverative tendencies were assessed by the Wisconsin card sorting test38 and scored for total errors, perseverative responses, and non-perseverative errors. The Beck depression inventory (BDI)39 was administered to quantify self-reported depression. A measure of verbal learning (California verbal learning test40) was administered to later patients but was unavailable for the preoperative assessment of controls or earlier patients performed at another institution; results are reported for the subset of pallidotomy patients for whom data were available. For the purpose of reporting the percentage of the preoperative sample that was impaired on each test, impairment was defined for an individual patient as a score falling more than 1.5 SD below the mean for that test using normative data from each instrument. This threshold was used based on conventional definitions of psychometric impairment.41

DATA ANALYSIS

The primary analysis was a series of one way (surgery/control) analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) on the postoperative neuropsychological scores using the baseline scores and retest interval in months as covariates. A power analysis from the ANCOVA shows that the effect size exceeds Cohen’s “large effect size” criterion of F=0.40 at a power of 0.80 and α of 0.05 using the numbers in the current sample.42 The number of patients in the control and surgery samples exceeding a decline of 1.5 SD units (measured at baseline across all patients) at retesting was tallied to examine individual changes, as opposed to the global changes assessed by parametric statistics calculated across individual patients. A pairedt test was used to examine changes from baseline to retesting for the ADL and motor scores from the UPDRS. These UPDRS scores were also correlated with neuropsychological test scores at baseline and follow up to determine if change in motor functions was associated with change in cognition.

Results

There were no significant differences between the 10 controls and 28 pallidotomy patients for any neuropsychological test at baseline or on retesting (table 1). None of the ANCOVA analyses was significant for differences between the pallidotomy and control groups. The baseline score covariate was significant in all analyses.

Mean (SD) scores by group and time tested

Examining individual patients, no control patient declined by more than 1.5 SD from baseline to follow up testing for any score. Two pallidotomy patients showed a decline of more than 1.5 SD units from baseline to follow up testing for the mini mental state examination (30 to 26 and 29 to 25), but neither had a postoperative score in the demented range. Two patients declined from baseline to follow up on the Stroop word score (T=46 to 20 and T=56 to 35). One pallidotomy patient declined on the controlled oral word association test (55 to 29). No patient declined on more than one test, and there was no clear pattern to the decline seen on single tests.

The total score from the BDI did not differ significantly between pallidotomy and control groups at baseline or follow up evaluations. The depression score did not correlate significantly with any cognitive measure at baseline or follow up. Only one surgery patient declined significantly on the BDI (0 to 13).

The UPDRS scores did not differ significantly at baseline between the pallidotomy group (ADL 17.4 (SD 7.8), motor 33.2 (SD 11.8)) and control group (ADL 17.8 (SD 8.8), motor 27.1 (SD 17.2)). The pallidotomy group improved significantly from baseline to postoperative testing for both the UPDRS ADL score (t=8.64, p<0.0001, postoperative ADL=6.6 (SD 5.9)) and the UPDRS motor score (t=13.37, p<0.0001, postoperative motor score=10.0 (SD 6.6)). The control group did not change significantly from baseline to follow up testing for either the ADL score (20.6 (SD 11.6)) or the motor score (31.6 (SD 16.2)). Neither the ADL score nor the motor score correlated significantly with any neuropsychological test at baseline or follow up, and the improvement in ADL and motor scores was not associated with change in the neuropsychological test scores.

Discussion

Pallidotomy did not result in significant deterioration in cognitive functioning. There was no differential change between the control and pallidotomy groups from baseline to retest for any discrete test of cognitive functioning. Only five pallidotomy patients declined by more than 1.5 SD from baseline to postoperative testing, and all five of these patients declined on only one score with no particular pattern to the changes. Depression as assessed by the BDI also showed no significant change between controls and pallidotomy patients, nor any change from baseline to follow up evaluation. There is thus no evidence that pallidotomy produces significant cognitive impairment.

Although some studies report postoperative deficits in language, visuospatial ability, or planning after pallidotomy or thalamotomy,43-48 methodological limitations restrict the confidence placed in the these older studies. Some utilised only postoperative data with no preoperative data,44 and the postoperative interval was short for most of the studies.43-45 Some studies combined pallidotomy and thalamotomy patients,43 45 48 and none had an adequate description of the operative procedure and specific target to determine the focus of the lesion. Most of the studies reported only global measures of functioning,44 45 47 without inclusion of specific tests of cognitive processing speed, concept formation, and visuospatial ability that are sensitive to the effects of Parkinson’s disease.6 Some of these older studies examined patients with large bilateral lesions that produced significant morbidity. By contrast, our findings suggest an absence of cognitive morbidity assessed by more discrete tests of cognitive functioning in patients with MRI guided stereotactic unilateral lesions.

Preoperative neuropsychological test scores showed that the patients with Parkinson’s disease in this study showed no significant dementia on a screening test, which reflects the exclusionary criteria of global dementia. However, many patients showed mild to moderate impairments in specific areas of cognitive functioning, including cognitive processing speed and visuospatial processing. These findings are consistent with previous neuropsychological findings in Parkinson’s disease showing no gross dementia for many patients6 21 48 but a preponderance of deficits consistent with frontal9 11 12 14 15 49 50 and right hemispheric dysfunction.6 7 9 16

Many patients also reported mild to moderate depression. Our incidence rate of 38.2% is comparable with the 46% rate reported in a review of 14 studies of Parkinson’s disease.51 However, depression ratings did not correlate significantly with cognitive ability. Other studies also show little relation between depression and degree of motor impairment52 or disability.52 53

Our findings suggest that unilateral pallidotomy in this subset of patients with refractory Parkinson’s disease exerts little impact on cognitive functioning. There was no deterioration between groups on any neuropsychological measure. No individual pallidotomy patient declined significantly on more than one test.

Both the pallidotomy and control groups had baseline and repeat testing to compare the groups on test-retest data to control for practice effects. However, the pallidotomy group had a longer mean follow up interval to retesting than the control group. If the lack of significant changes were due to practice effects, then the control group should have improved more than the pallidotomy group due to the shorter test to retest interval. Because the groups did not differ significantly in changes from baseline to retesting, it is unlikely that the findings obtained are due to simple practice effects. The control group was self selected by declining to submit to surgery or postponing surgery, and an argument could be made that this group differed from the group of patients who underwent surgery. However, analyses showed no difference between these two groups for severity of motor symptoms, medication, or cognitive ability. Although some systematic bias between the control and operative groups cannot be completely ruled out, there was no obvious difference in the primary variables of interest in this study.

Unilateral stereotactic ventral pallidotomy thus seems to have little associated postoperative cognitive morbidity. However, our sample was restricted to patients with lateralised motor symptoms and significant fluctuations between “on” dyskinesias and “off” parkinsonism. The effects of pallidotomy in less lateralised patients and the effects of bilateral pallidotomy on cognitive function remain unknown.

References