Article Text

Download PDFPDF

Letter
Behavioural symptoms in progressive supranuclear palsy and frontotemporal dementia
  1. Thomas H Bak1,2,
  2. Linda M Crawford2,
  3. German Berrios3,
  4. John R Hodges4,5
  1. 1Department of Psychology, Edinburgh University, Edinburgh, UK
  2. 2Medical Research Council—Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK
  3. 3University Department of Psychiatry, Addenbrooke's Hospital, Cambridge, UK
  4. 4University Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Addenbrooke's Hospital, Cambridge, UK
  5. 5Prince of Wales Medical Research Institute, Sydney, Australia
  1. Correspondence to Professor John Hodges, Prince of Wales Medical Research Institute, Cnr Barker Street and Easy Street, Randwick, New South Wales, Australia, 2031; j.hodges{at}powmri.edu.au

Statistics from Altmetric.com

Request Permissions

If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.

Within the spectrum of frontotemporal dementia (FTD), the behavioural variant (bvFTD) presents with prominent changes in personality, most notably disinhibition, loss of empathy, stereotypic behaviour, alteration of food preference and apathy.1 These features have been linked to orbitofrontal and mesial-cingulate pathology.2

In comparison, less is known about the behavioural changes in progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP) which remain in the shadow of the motor, oculo-motor and cognitive symptoms. In one study, PSP patients were found to be apathetic and disinhibited, while the Parkinson's disease patients had a higher frequency of hallucinations, delusions and depression.3 Imaging studies in PSP have demonstrated frontal-lobe atrophy that correlates with the degree of executive dysfunction and behavioural change on the frontal behavioural inventory.4 We compared large cohorts of patients with PSP and bvFTD using a recently validated instrument, the Cambridge Behavioural Inventory.5

A total of 185 patients with PSP participated (100 men, 85 women, mean age: 69.3±6.6 years). Two hundred and ten care-givers of patients with PSP were contacted by the PSP Association; 135 agreed to participate (a response rate of 64%). A further 50 patients were diagnosed in Cambridge. For those ascertained via the PSP Association, we included a symptom checklist which was reviewed to confirm that the clinical profile was typical of PSP. Comparison of demographic features, length of history and Cambridge Behavioural Inventory (CBI) scores showed no differences between the subgroups which were, therefore, combined. The PSP cohort was subdivided according to the disease duration into four groups: 1–2 years (31 patients), 3–4 years (50 patients), 5–6 years (37 patients) and over 7 years (37 patients). The bvFTD cohort consisted of 47 patients (34 men, 13 women; mean age: 60.8±6.7 years) from Cambridge. The study was approved by the Cambridgeshire Research Ethics Committee.

The CBI, available from http://www.ftdrg.org, is a care-giver-based questionnaire4 that has been validated against the Neuropsychiatric Inventory and distinguishes characteristic profiles in FTD, Parkinson's disease and Alzheimer's disease.4 The CBI includes questions covering: mood (depression, elation), psychotic symptoms (delusions, hallucinations), challenging behaviour and aggression, disinhibition (including sexually inappropriate behaviour), eating habits, stereotypic behaviours, sleep and motivation (loss of interests and drive, social withdrawal). Care-givers rate the frequency of each behaviour over the past month on a scale from 0 (never) to 4 (constantly). In keeping with other studies,4 we dichotomised the data: 0–2 were considered as low, and 3 and 4 as high endorsement. Differences in distribution were analysed using χ2 tests with Bonferroni corrections.

Figure 1A illustrates the distribution of high endorsement for individual CBI subtests in both patient groups. Poor motivation (apathy) was the most prevalent feature in bvFTD (70%) and was also endorsed to a high degree in PSP (35%). The bvFTD patients scored significantly higher than the PSP group (p<0.05) on all subtests, except psychotic symptoms and sleep disturbance. On the sleep subtest, the PSP patients were significantly more impaired, while psychotic symptoms were virtually absent in both groups. No significant relationship was found in either group between any of the subtests and either age or gender. As shown in figure 1B, the only subtests in which there was a significant relationship between disease duration and high endorsement were motivation and sleep, which both increased in a linear fashion with disease duration (p<0.05).

Figure 1

(A) Comparison of progressive supranuclear palsy (PSP) and frontotemporal dementia (FTD) patients with high endorsement on the individual Cambridge Behavioural Inventory (CBI) subtests. (B) Distribution of high CBI endorsement in PSP patients with different disease duration.

The most characteristic behavioural features found in PSP, and its main similarity to bvFTD, were the high frequency and severity of the apathy, which occurred despite the relatively low scores on the mood subtest. Our results confirm, therefore, the view that the apathetic syndrome in PSP constitutes an independent entity that cannot simply be accounted for by depression.

Although the CBI correlates well with the Neuropsychiatric Inventory4 and appears to discern changes in motivation from mood disturbance, it remains possible that physical immobility contributes to the apparent apathy in PSP.

The most striking difference between the PSP and the bvFTD groups was the much lower frequency of disinhibition, aggression, stereotypic behaviour and alterations in eating habits in the PSP cohort. It is unlikely that this difference was related to different disease severity, as the frequency of these symptoms did not increase in PSP with disease duration, in contrast to apathy, which showed a linear increase. Positive behavioural symptoms, of the type found in bvFTD, do not seem, therefore, to be linked to the progression of the disease. This observation argues in favour of a qualitative, rather than quantitative, difference between the condition. Neither group exhibited significant psychotic symptoms. Although delusions and hallucinations have been reported in FTD,1 they are uncommon.

The only subtest on which the PSP patients had significantly higher endorsement than the bvFTD cohort was sleep disturbance which, like motivation, showed a linear increase with disease duration. Unfortunately, the data available did not allow us to explore the nature of the sleep disturbance in more detail.

The observed differential pattern of behavioural symptoms in PSP and bvFTD is likely to reflect differences in the distribution of pathology. The pathology of bvFTD is believed to start in the orbitofrontal and mesial region, causing deficits in social cognition, emotion recognition, moral judgement and decision-making. Apathy, by contrast, is typically associated with mesial frontal pathology. The very high rate of apathy in PSP suggests that this region is dysfunctional as a result of either direct involvement or deafferentation secondary to subcortical pathology.3

The findings are of practical relevance in that care-givers can be reassured that the disease is unlikely to lead to aggressive, challenging, unpredictable or dangerous behaviours, which can be so distressing to those involved in care of bvFTD patients. However, the recognition of apathy as an integral, organically explainable feature of the disease, rather than a sign of rejection or disinterest, might reassure the care-givers and help them to develop better coping strategies.

References

Footnotes

  • Funding This study would not have been possible without the support of the PSP Association. JRH is currently supported by an Australian Research Council Federation Fellowship.

  • Competing interests None.

  • Patient consent Obtained.

  • Ethics approval Ethics approval was provided by the Cambridgeshire Research Ethics Committee.

  • Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.