Article Text
Abstract
A 67 year old right handed Japanese man developed prosopagnosia caused by a haemorrhage. His only deficit was the inability to perceive and discriminate unfamiliar faces, and to recognise familiar faces. He did not show deficits in visual or visuospatial perception of non-facial stimuli, alexia, visual agnosia, or topographical disorientation. Brain MRI showed a haematoma limited to the right fusiform and the lateral occipital region. Single photon emission computed tomography confirmed that there was no decreased blood flow in the opposite left cerebral hemisphere. The present case indicates that a well placed small right fusiform gyrus and the adjacent area can cause isolated impairment of facial recognition. As far as we know, there has been no published case that has demonstrated this exact lesion site, which was indicated by recent functional MRI studies as the most critical area in facial recognition.
- prosopagnosia
- magnetic resonance imaging
- single photon emission computed tomography
- right fusiform gyrus
Statistics from Altmetric.com
- prosopagnosia
- magnetic resonance imaging
- single photon emission computed tomography
- right fusiform gyrus
Prosopagnosia is a unique syndrome in which recognition of familiar faces is impaired. Recent reports have found it to occur in patients with lesions confined to the right temporo-occipital region.1As the restricted unilateral lesion in most of these previous cases were demonstrated by CT, it is possible that other lesions existed that might have escaped visualisation. Few cases of prosopagnosia have been reported in which the lesions were confined to the right hemisphere by MRI, SPECT, or PET.1 2 However, these cases exhibited visually related dysfunction as well as prosopagnosia, such as topographical disorientation, alexia, and visual object agnosia. So there remains an uncertainty as to the critical lesion responsible for prosopagnosia. We herein report a case of the selective impairment of face recognition after a haemorrhage confined to the right fugiform and lateral occipital region, confirmed by MRI.
Case report
The patient was a 67 year old, right handed, retired journalist with a history of non-systemic vasculitic polyneuropathy 3 years previously. He experienced a sudden disturbance of the visual field and became aware of his inability to recognise the faces of family members and even thought that his own reflection in the mirror was the face of a stranger. Two days later, when he came to our hospital, he was unable to recognise his doctor, but knew him by his voice. At that time, brain CT disclosed a haemorrhage in the inferolateral part of the right occipital lobe.
He was alert and oriented. He initially had a mild left homonymous hemianopia that vanished within 2 weeks (Goldmann visual field was normal). Visual acuity was 1.0 (20/20) in both eyes. Colour vision was intact with Ishihara colour plates (no achromatopsia). Ocular movements were full. The remaining cranial nerves were intact. He had remaining bilateral sensory deficit and muscle weakness in the distal parts of his four limbs due to non-systemic vasculitic polyneuropathy. No dysmetria was found. Tendon reflexes were absent and Babinski's sign was negative on both sides.
He could no longer recognise his wife or other familial people when he saw their faces. He said that he would usually identify family members and friends by non-facial, distinguishing features. He was also unable to recognise photographed faces of famous people. For example, confronted with a photograph of the Japanese sumo champion, Akebono, in front of the Meiji shrine, he could not recognise his face, but immediately named the shrine correctly.
During 3 months after the onset, the degree of his impairment in face recognition remained so severe as to interfere with daily life. Thereafter his symptoms improved to some extent, but he still reported that he was often unable to recognise the faces of famous people photographed in the newspaper. We followed him up for more than 6 months and his symptoms still remained.
BRAIN IMAGING
Brain MRI was carried out 27 days after onset. The lesion involved the right inferior occipital region at the level of the fusiform gyrus, and inferior and fourth occipital gyri, but spared the striate, lingual cortices, and parahippocampal gyrus (fig 1). N-isopropyl-(123I)-p-iodoamphetamine SPECT disclosed decreased blood flow corresponding to the haemorrhage.
Coronal view of the T1 weighted MR images (TM/TE= 300/ 4.2). The images show a high intensity area in the right fusiform gyrus and lateral occipital region.
GENERAL COGNITIVE FUNCTIONING
He was alert and fully oriented to time and space, obtaining a score of 30/30 on the mini mental state examination. There was no personal change or apparent intellectual deterioration. No deficit of language or praxis was seen.3 He scored 32/36 on the Raven coloured progressive matrice (normal range). He had no memory impairment and his memory index on the Wechsler memory scale-revised (WMS-R) were normal (general memory index 96; visual memory index 104; verbal memory index 91; delayed memory index 91). Reading ability was also intact.
EXPERIMENTAL INVESTIGATIONS
Visual perception tests
He never complained of difficulty in recognising objects and places, or finding his way around places. General visual perception and visual construction were tested using the visual perception test for agnosia (VPTA),4 which is a test standardised in Japan, designed to assess higher order visual perceptual abilities. As shown in table 1, in VPTA, he performed at a normal level in all categories except for the face recognition task. He showed various deficits in the face recognition task except for perception of expression task.
Patient's performance on tests
In addition to VPTA, his visual performance was assessed with a battery of tests of increasing difficulty. The detail of methods and items are listed in the .
Methods
VISUAL PERCEPTION TESTS EXCEPT VPTA
- (1)
- Naming and recognition of line drawings; we used the method of Kazui et al. 5
- (2)
- Picture naming (conventional and unconventional views); the photographs of 10 common objects were used.
- (3)
- Naming and recognition of famous places; 12 photographs were used.
- (4)
- Naming of animal faces; the photographs of six animals were used.
- (5)
- Face matching.
- (6)
- Memory of faces; for (5) and (6) () we used the method of Kazui et al.5
- (7)
- Identification of familiar faces: a series of nine photographs were used.
- (8)
- Famous face identification; the photographs of 38 famous faces were used.
- (9)
- Identification from name; we investigated whether he had an intact knowledge of famous people.
Results
As shown in table 1, he responded correctly on visual perception tests and thus, there was no evidence of any significant decline in visuoperceptual abilities. By contrast, he showed various deficits in tests of face processing. In the face matching test, his performance was slow but within the normal range. In the memory faces test, he showed a mild deficit. In the familiar faces identification test, he was able to identify only two out of 11 faces, and for most of the rest he responded that he had no idea. Of nine faces that he could not identify, he reported that only one face was encountered before. It was noted that he could neither identify nor had familiarity with his own face. In famous faces identification, he showed a severe deficit. By contrast, by identification from name, he was able to provide detailed information correctly.
Discussion
After extensive testing, our patient was found to manifest impairment restricted to the recognition of faces, due to the small lesion in the right fusiform and lateral occipital region. He demonstrated severe prosopagnosia, yet he showed no clear deficit in visual or visuospatial perception of non-facial stimuli. He showed no other abnormal findings that could possibly cause a disturbance of face recognition, such as dementia or general memory disturbance. He could also recognise common visual objects shown from both canonical and non-canonical perspectives on photographs. So far in the literature, all prosopagnosic patients who had been subjected to a detailed examination had displayed visual perceptual deficits of non-facial stimuli. Landis et al reported a prosopagnosic patient who was the first patient studied postmortem.6 They also reported that their patient was unable to identify the items when presented non-canonically. Sergent and Signoret also reported on three prosopagnosic patients who were all unable to recognise objects seen from a non-canoninal perspective.7 However, Warrington and Taylor had already pointed out that this deficit was typically associated with the right inferior parietal lesion.8 Similarly to our patient, patient OR reported by De Renzi et al 1 scored normally on tests for the objects presented from atypical views. Thus these findings provide evidence that prosopagnosia and deficits in recognising objects shown from unconventional views are distinct signs and the last is not necessarily associated with prosopagnosia.
Our patient was impaired in the task of learning unknown faces despite normal visual memory. Our data were in agreement with the contention of Takahashi et al 9 that the impairment in the ability to memorise new faces was a common feature of prosopagnosia. However, their patients were impaired on Benton's visual retention test. This dissociation in our patient's visual memory emphasised that the impairment of our patient was face specific.
Our search in the literature over the past 25 years has shown that there were only nine prosopagnosic patients so far, whose damage was confined to the right hemisphere as shown by MRI, and 10 patients by PET.1 2 9 The lesion in our patient was localised in the right fusiform, inferior, and fourth occipital gyri. Takahashiet al 9 suggested that the cortical/subcortical damage to the lingual and fusiform gyri play the most important part in prosopagnosia. However, similarly to our patient, the lingual cortex was spared in patient PC reported on by Sergent and Signoret.7 We think, therefore, that the fusiform gyrus is mainly involved in the recognition of the face.
Recent investigations of face perception in normal subjects using functional imaging techniques identified the right fusiform gyrus, and proposed that this region is primarily involved in face perception.10-14 Our present finding is consistent with these functional imaging results.
Acknowledgments
We are very grateful to Dr Y Eguchi, at the Graduate School of Human and Environmental Studies, Kyoto University, for the generous gift of material of tests, and thank Dr Yasushi Kita, and Hisako Saida, STR, at the Department of Neurology, Osaka Saiseikai Nakatsu Hospital.
Lists of methods and items used in the tests of visual perception and Investigation of face processing.
(1) NAMING AND RECOGNITION OF LINE DRAWINGS
The test stimuli were the 90 line drawings from Kazuiet al,5 which consisted of nine diverse categories (vehicles, musical instruments, animals, processed food, utensils, vegetables/fruits, sports, body parts, and colours). Each picture was presented, one at a time, with the instructions to name each object as it was presented. He was later given a recognition test, usually during a separate session. In this case each of the stimulus pictures was presented along with nine others drawn from the same category. Instructions were simply to point to the picture named. Recognition was tested for all pictures regardless whether or not he had been correctly named in the earlier session.
(2) PICTURE NAMING (CONVENTIONAL AND UNCONVENTIONAL VIEW)
Photographs of 10 common objects taken both from a conventional and an unconventional view were presented for naming, following the procedure devised by Wallington and Taylor.10Unconventional views were given first.
(3) NAMING AND RECOGNITION OF FAMOUS PLACES
He was shown a set of photographs of 12 famous Japanese places including famous sites and historical monuments (for example, Mount Fuji, Kiyomizu temple) and asked to provide the name or as much identifying information as possible. On another occasion he was provided with the name of each place and asked to provide as much information about it as possible.
(4) NAMING OF ANIMAL FACES
The photographs of the head regions of six well known animals were shown to the patient, and he was asked to provide the name or as much identifying information as possible.
(5) FACE MATCHING
We used the method of Kazui et al 5, constructed in analogy with the test of Benton and Van Allen.15 A total of 21 photographs of Japanese were prepared, seven being frontal views, seven three quarter views, and seven lateral views. He was requested to match these faces with those of other views.
(6) MEMORY OF FACES
Following the method of Kazui et al,5 the ability to recognise faces (including hair) was tested with 18 photographs of Japanese women. Firstly, photographs of nine women were shown and then he was asked to recognise them from the full set of 18 photographs, presented after a delay of 15 minutes.
(7) IDENTIFICATION OF FAMILIAR FACES
He was shown a series of nine photographs of the patient himself and doctors or hospital staff who have been familiar with him for more than 2 years, as well as two photographs of the doctor and nurse whom he had often seen subsequent to the onset of haemorrhage. He was asked to identify these faces. If unable to name the person, he was asked to provide as much identifying information as he could. The score used here was the number of faces he could name or provide sufficient information to uniquely identify the person. If unable to identify them, he was asked to make a yes/no judgement as to whether or not he thought the face was encountered previously.
(8) FAMOUS FACE IDENTIFICATION
He was shown 38 famous faces (including 25 politicians) selected from a remote memory test, using the stimuli of Eguchiet al from the 1970s-90s.16 The procedure of this test was the same as that for identification in the familiar face test.
(9) IDENTIFICATION FROM NAME
We investigated whether he had intact knowledge of people by giving him the name of the 38 famous people whose photographs were used in the famous face identification test. He was asked to provide as much information as possible.